Научная статья на тему 'THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK, MOTIVATION AND EFL ACHIEVEMENT IN MIDDLE AND HIGH SCHOOL EDUCATION'

THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK, MOTIVATION AND EFL ACHIEVEMENT IN MIDDLE AND HIGH SCHOOL EDUCATION Текст научной статьи по специальности «Языкознание и литературоведение»

CC BY-ND
146
42
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Ключевые слова
CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK / MOTIVATION / STUDENTS’ ACHIEVEMENT / ATTITUDES

Аннотация научной статьи по языкознанию и литературоведению, автор научной работы — Ahmetovic Emnijeta, Becirovic Senad, Dubravac Vildana, Brdarevic-Celjo Amna

Introduction. Despite the fact that error correction has significant and long-term effects on facilitating language learning and development, there has not been any research that investigates its influence on learners’ motivation within the classroom context of Bosnia and Herzegovina. Purpose. This research aims to examine the impact of written and oral corrective feedback on students’ motivation and achievement within this EFL context. Method. For this quantitative study, the questionnaire has been used to collect the data from 160 middle and high school students in central Bosnia and Herzegovina. Results. The findings indicated that the respondents generally like to be corrected and they are moderately to highly motivated to speak and write in English as a foreign language. Furthermore, learners with positive attitudes towards the received feedback feel significantly more motivated to keep learning than those with negative attitudes. Conclusion. The study is expected to provide teachers with suggestions on how to transform their classrooms into an environment conducive to the development of higher levels of writing and speaking motivation and how to provide corrective feedback that will positively influence students’ EFL achievement.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK, MOTIVATION AND EFL ACHIEVEMENT IN MIDDLE AND HIGH SCHOOL EDUCATION»

https://doi.org/10.17323/jle.2023.12663

The Interplay between Corrective Feedback, Motivation and EFL Achievement in Middle and High School Education

Emnijeta Ahmetovic Senad Becirovic Vildana Dubravac Amna Brdarevic-Celjo ®

International Burch University, Sarajevo, Bosnia and Herzegovina

ABSTRACT

Introduction. Despite the fact that error correction has significant and long-term effects on facilitating language learning and development, there has not been any research that investigates its influence on learners' motivation within the classroom context of Bosnia and Herzegovina.

Purpose. This research aims to examine the impact of written and oral corrective feedback on students' motivation and achievement within this EFL context.

Method. For this quantitative study, the questionnaire has been used to collect the data from 160 middle and high school students in central Bosnia and Herzegovina.

Results. The findings indicated that the respondents generally like to be corrected and they are moderately to highly motivated to speak and write in English as a foreign language. Furthermore, learners with positive attitudes towards the received feedback feel significantly more motivated to keep learning than those with negative attitudes.

Conclusion. The study is expected to provide teachers with suggestions on how to transform their classrooms into an environment conducive to the development of higher levels of writing and speaking motivation and how to provide corrective feedback that will positively influence students' EFL achievement.

KEYWORDS

corrective feedback, motivation, students' achievement, attitudes

INTRODUCTION

Citation: Ahmetovic E., Becirovic S., Dubravac V., & Brdarevic-Celjo A. (2023). The Interplay between Corrective Feedback, Motivation and EFL Achievement in Middle and High School Education. Journal of Language and Education, 9(1), 26-40. https://doi. org/10.17323/jle.2023.12663

Correspondence:

Emnijeta Ahmetovic, emnijeta.ahmetovic@stu.ibu.edu.ba

Received: June 29, 2021 Accepted: Mach 15, 2023 Published: March 31, 2023

Learning motivation is learners' desire or willingness to put time and effort into initiating or sustaining the process of language learning (Ortega, 2009) and can eventually determine their success or failure in learning a foreign language. Thus, motivated individuals will learn a foreign language at a faster pace and to a larger degree than individuals lacking motivation (Gass & Selinker, 2001, p. 349). As such, motivation has attracted a keen interest of researchers (e.g. Deci & Ryan, 1991;Dornyei, 2000;Gardner & MacIntyre, 1993), who have tried to develop a suitable model explaining the role of motivation in the process of second language learning. One of these models is the Self-Determination Theory which takes human interaction with the

social environment as a way of satisfying the needs of autonomy, competence and relatedness as its starting point (Lagaut, 2017). Thus, if learners perform an activity with an internal drive for knowledge, pleasure and feeling of satisfaction, they are considered to be intrinsically motivated. Conversely, if the reason for doing the task is controlled from the outside world by the means of rewards and punishments, that is referred to as extrinsic motivation.

However, as indicated by previous researchers, the construct of motivation is a dynamic variable (Dervic & Becirovic, 2019; Dornyei, 2005; Masic et al., 2020), and is influenced by other individual and environmental factors, one of which is corrective feedback (CF) (Vasquez & Harvey, 2010) or a teacher's response

to learners' incorrect utterances of some sort. Since CF occurs in a response to learners' errors, it is believed to be beneficial to students because it gives them negative evidence and helps them to see the difference between the target language they have acquired and the target-like L2 (Second Language) form (Long, 1996). Russell and Spada (2006) maintained that CF in L2 classroom instruction is an apt tool for improving language production despite the existing differences in teachers' instruction modes. Still, in spite of such positive perceptions of CF as a practice prompting successful L2 acquisition (e.g., Doughty & Long, 2003; Doughty & Williams, 1998; Gass, 2003; Long, 1996; Schachter, 1991), it is very frequently observed as a needless practice with little impact on L2 learning (e.g., Flynn, 1996). Nevertheless, in teaching schemes, feedback plays an essential role in maintaining the teacher-student or peer-student interaction (Polz & Becirovic, 2022; Hyland & Hyland, 2006) and also in enhancing students' autonomous learning (Hyland, 2003) and its efficacy relies on the nature of linguistic features, teaching practices, and learners' individual characteristics, motivation in particular (Schachter, 1991).

CF has also been frequently pointed out as a factor which can negatively influence motivation (DeKeyser, 1993; Jean & Simard, 2011), but also as a process appreciated by language learners and observed as a factor strengthening motivation (Jean & Simard, 2011; Lee, 2013; Yoshida, 2008). In the majority of recent studies feedback is seen as beneficial (Evans et al., 2010; Ferris, 2006; Kang & Han, 2015; Nassaji, 2016). However, there is still some controversy about what form of correction is more influential (Al-Rubai'ey & Nassaji, 2013; Bitchener, 2008; Chen et al., 2016; Nassaji, 2015), which has proved to be at least partially dependent on learners' individual differences (Zamel, 1985), and their interest in the learning process (Papi et al., 2019).

Hence, teachers should have a deep insight into theoretical as well as practical guidelines related to this issue (Schulz, 2001), and they are expected to take learners' beliefs into account as well (Jean & Simard, 2011; Yaman & Becirovic, 2016), since the difference between their and their students' beliefs could have detrimental effects on motivation (Kalaja & Barcelos, 2003). Therefore, to foster learners' motivation and ensure linguistic accuracy, teachers need to be familiar with their students' perceptions of the best way of correcting mistakes. In order to be extrinsically motivated, students need encouragement and support which can be provided by teachers through feedback as a reward. However, if learners are aware of their own learning progress, it can lead to greater intrinsic motivation and students' engagement in learning. Thus, as found by previous researchers (e.g., Choi, 2013; Gardner, 2011), the use of CF might decrease the risk of lowering students' motivation.

The studies determining how and to what extent CF and motivation interact with each other to affect language

learning outcomes in a foreign language learning context seem to be relatively scant. Moreover, it has also been pointed out that such studies have mainly focused on adults and that there is a need for research revealing the impact of CF on younger learners (Jean & Simard, 2011). Therefore, the current study aims to investigate the relationship between CF and motivation taking into account their interactive effect on middle and high school students' English language performance. Moreover, it aims to help teachers, students, and future researchers understand whether the way of providing feedback in middle and high school motivates students and to what degree. It also reveals students' attitudes towards the received feedback as well as to its frequency, and the impact of error correction on students' achievement. Such a comprehensive approach can further our understanding of why CF is so important and can open new ways of research on how to make feedback more effective in order to motivate students.

LITERATURE REVIEW

Corrective Feedback

Although research into the role of CF and its impact on L2 acquisition is steadily progressing, there is no single study that covers all aspects of understanding its controversial function. In general, CF has been found beneficial to SLA (Second Language Acquisition) but in order to gain a better understanding of its controversial role as well as to identify research orientations for L2 scholars, it is important to present a comprehensive overview of its effectiveness.

The usefulness of both written and oral correction for learners' L2 improvement has been frequently confirmed (Ellis, 2009; Li & Vuono, 2019) and negative evidence has emerged as a necessary factor for language learning advancement (Gooch et al., 2016; Li, 2010; Lyster & Saito, 2010; Lyster et al., 2013; Nassaji, 2016, 2017; Yang & Lyster, 2010). Several recent meta-analyses (e.g., Kang & Han, 2015; Karim & Nassaji, 2019; Lyster & Saito, 2010; Plonsky & Brown, 2015; Russell & Spada, 2006) have also shown that CF significantly impacts L2 learners' performance, with long-lasting effects (Russel & Spada, 2006). Correction frequently appears in numerous classroom events (Brown, 2016; Wang & Li, 2020) and it has been particularly effective in constituting students' achievement (Bitchener et al., 2005; Choi, 2013; Li & Vuono, 2019; Sheen, 2007). Patra et al. (2022) showed that immediate feedback positively affects learners' academic performance, since it triggers positive attitudes towards continuous learning and learners get more involved in learning.

Some researchers find oral correction feedback unneeded and useless. According to Krashen (1982), error treatment is maleficent to the whole learning process, as it makes learners avoid complex tasks, and "even under the best

of conditions, with the most learning-oriented students, teacher corrections will not produce results that will live up to the expectations of many instructors" (Krashen, 1982, p. 119). Truscott (1999) believes that correction provided during spoken activities should be avoided as it does not enhance learners' willingness to speak accurately and mentions some of its disadvantages, such as the difficulty of giving clear explanation for an error and the uncertainty as to whether feedback will help learners in acquiring the target language. Lyster et al. (1999) disagree with Truscott's findings, claiming that effectiveness of oral CF is certain and correction is necessary in some cases. Through correction, students engage in their L2 "communicatively-authentic discourse" (Lyster et al., 2013), which in return improves their accuracy, fluency as well as automaticity (Saito, 2021, p. 408). Both students and teachers in Ha et al. (2021) study conducted in the Vietnamese EFL context felt very positive about explicit feedback types, namely explicit correction and metalinguistic feedback, but they differed in their beliefs on feedback timing. While the students favor immediate feedback, the teachers worry about their emotional state and are concerned that feedback may disrupt the flow of their speech (Ha et al., 2021).

In addition to studies investigating spoken error correction, there have been a few studies investigating whether CF of written errors can help students to improve their writing accuracy. In some earlier studies, corrective feedback of written errors did not prove particularly useful as the students who received correction and those who did not differed insignificantly in writing accuracy (e.g. Robb et al., 1986; Sheppard, 1992). In some more recent studies, feedback has proved to be beneficial to students' writing accuracy (Bitchener, 2017; Bitchener & Storch, 2016; Bruton, 2010; Chandler, 2009; Chen & Nassaji, 2018; Ferris 2006; Kang & Han, 2015;Karim & Nassaji, 2019), though the degree to which it can improve L2 writing is still debatable (Karim & Nassaji, 2019). Karim and Nassaji (2019) also emphasized that students need to provide output with the correct form on their own as such practices could be more useful for their interlanguage development. Several studies have investigated the implementation of written corrective feedback (WCF) over a longer time period (i.e., weeks or months) and have come to different conclusions. Some studies provide support for WCF (see. Ashwell, 2000; Ferris & Roberts, 2001), while some others fail to see any advantages on the side of the group that received WCF (Polio et al., 1998). This might be assigned to the non-existence of a control group in some previous studies (Bitchener, 2008; Storch, 2010; as cited in Karim & Nassaji, 2019) or to the measurement of the usefulness of correction only in terms of the production of revised utterances (e.g., Ferris, 2010). More recent studies have investigated the effect of correction not only on revised but also on new pieces of writing (Hartshorn et al., 2010; Karim & Nassaji, 2018; Lopez et al., 2018) and their findings varied depending upon the ways of providing feedback. Xu's (2021) findings showed

that Chinese learners were generally well-disposed towards WCF in an online environment in this case, but what they particularly liked is the clarity of teachers' feedback and its potential for being analyzed further or as marked in their responses 'indefinitely' (Xu, 2021, p. 568).

When grading students' written compositions, teachers are obliged to provide correction but are also advised to provide feedback "an objective that may operate at cross-purposes with the evaluative goal" (Hedgcock & Lefkowitz, 1996, p. 288). Teachers sometimes tend to be impacted by the belief that the more errors they correct the more responsible teachers they are (Lee, 2013, p. 113), which is potentially dangerous and can impact the learning process (Hyland & Hyland, 2006). It is generally suggested that not all errors should be corrected but correction should be directed towards a few errors so that learners pay more attention to the feedback thus provided (Ellis et al., 2008).

Corrective Feedback and Motivation

Although it has been confirmed that both feedback and motivation are important for language learning, empirically very little is known about their interplay in SLA (Ellis, 2010). More specifically, some recent studies conducted by the leading CF investigators (Lyster et al., 2013; Nassaji, 2016) do not pay so much attention to the impact of motivation on the usefulness of correction (Bitchener, 2017) and neither do L2 motivation investigators focus so much on this relation (see Dornyei & Ushioda, 2011).

However, numerous experimental and non-experimental research studies have found CF and motivation to be positively correlated (e.g., DeKeyser 1993; Guilloteaux & Dornyei, 2008). Dornyei and Csizer (1998) interviewed 200 Hungarian language teachers asking them to rank a selection of 51 teaching strategies considered as motivational factors and the results showed that one of the strategies ranked high was teacher feedback which is seen as an influential factor in students' motivation and as a stimulator of learners' motivation and self-confidence. Hence, consistent feedback is considered to be essential in facilitating L2 motivation since it carries a clear message about the teacher's priorities and is reflected in the student motivation (Dornyei, 1994, p. 278). A similar study conducted by Guilloteaux & Dornyei (2008) among EFL students in South Korea also indicated that CF in the form of prompts and students' motivation for language learning are strongly and positively correlated.

The correlation between CF and motivation was also found in a quasi-experimental study carried out by DeKeyser (1993), which examined the influence of feedback on L2 grammar and oral performance. In this study, the participants were learners of French who went through a ten-month treatment in Belgium, with one group obtaining constant correction and the other group not obtaining any. DeKeyser (1993) found that students with lower motivation achieved better results

on oral fluency tests as well as accuracy tests after constantly receiving correction, which suggests that correction might directly affect motivation and eventually have positive outcomes for the learning process. Thus, DeKeyser's original hypothesis stating that students with low motivation would take CF as criticism and that it would not be beneficial for them as much as for students with high motivation was refuted since the results confirmed that correction had an impact on students' L2 motivation regardless of its initial level. Likewise, Hamidun et al. (2012) examined if teacher's feedback can foster students' motivation. As it was initially found through classroom observation that students were not motivated or had little motivation to engage in language activities, explicit correction was used, and the results indicated that direct feedback increased the level of motivation, with participants responding well to this type of error correction. Al-Darei & Ahmed (2022) also researched the effect of feedback but also of feedback type on students' motivation and achievement in the e-learning environment. Their findings also indicated an increase in motivation and achievement after the implementation of feedback, particularly interpretative feedback.

The Present Study

The present study aims to explore whether written and oral corrective feedback lead to an increase in students' motivation for learning English as a foreign language and better EFL achievement in the middle- and high-school milieu of Bosnia and Herzegovina. Based on the theoretical and empirical findings presented above, the research will respond to the following research questions:

(1) Does oral CF significantly correlate with motivation for EFL speaking in the classroom?

(2) Does written CF significantly correlate with motivation for EFL writing in the classroom?

(3) Does gender significantly influence motivation for EFL speaking and writing when the English study duration is controlled?

(4) Does oral and written CF in English as a foreign language significantly predict the students' EFL achievement?

METHOD

Participants

The research sample consisted of 160 middle and high school students. Different grade levels were included with randomly selected participants, and a stratified sampling method was employed. Two classes (the first and the third grade) from high school and five classes (from the fifth to

the ninth grade) from middle school, both located in central Bosnia, participated in this research. The sample consisted of 90 female participants (56.3%) and 70 male participants (43.8%) aged between 10 to 17 (M = 13.83, SD = 1.998). Table 1 gives an accurate description of the representative

selection. Table 1

Descriptive Analysis of the Research Sample

_N_%_

School Middle school 118 73.8

High school 42 26.3

Gender Female participants 90 56.3

Male participants 70 43.8

Fifth 16 10.0

Sixth 16 10.0

Seventh 29 18.1

Grade Eighth 27 16.9 levels

Ninth 30 18.8

First 21 13.1

Third 21 13.1

Total 160 100.0

Instruments and Procedures

After the schools' administration as well as the participants themselves granted the approval, the data collection tools were prepared, administrated and adjusted accordingly to the middle and high school by the researchers themselves. The participants were informed that the data obtained from these surveys will be anonymous, confidential and voluntary and were provided with an appropriate clarification on how to fill in a Likert-type scale.

The questionnaire comprised three core parts. The first part collected statistical data about the participants' individual characteristics, such as gender, age, GPA in English, grade level and the duration of English language learning process. The second part consisted of the motivation scale to speak and write in English as a foreign language designed and authorized by Ryan & Connell (1989). The aim of this survey was to gain more in-depth information about how participants are motivated to speak and write in English as well as its correlation with other variables such as gender, achievement and the duration of learning English. The instrument comprised 33 items for speaking and 33 items for writing motivation, separated into three

crucial categories, namely intrinsic, extrinsic motivation and amotivation, which are further divided into seven subscales (amotivation e.g. Doing these activities is not interesting for me; external regulation e.g. Because I know I will get in trouble if I do not; introjected regulation e.g. Because it is absolutely necessary to do these activities if one wants to be successful in language learning; identified regulation e.g. Because it is a good way to gain lots of skills that could be useful to me in other areas of language learning and my life; intrinsic motivation for knowledge e.g. Because I experience a great pleasure while discovering new techniques of expression of ideas and feelings through these activities; intrinsic motivation for accomplishment e.g. Because I think carrying out hard tasks in these activities will improve my performance; and intrinsic motivation for stimulation e.g. Because it makes me happy). The Relative Autonomy Index (RAI) was used to measure both self-determined motivation for speaking and writing by using the original RAI formula: RAI = (2 * intrinsic + identified) - (2 * external + introjected) (Ünlü, 2016, 2019). The instrument showed the overall consistency reliability, motivation to write, in total (introjected regulation, identified regulation, intrinsic motivation for knowledge, intrinsic motivation for accomplishment and intrinsic motivation for stimulation dimension) a = .81 as well as motivation to speak, in total a = .79. Also, the compounded writing intrinsic motivation a = .75 and speaking intrinsic motivation a = .72 were consistent.

The third and the last part of the survey was the Corrective Feedback Scale, adopted from Calderón (2013). It employed a five-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree and it contained 15 statements related to written feedback and 15 statements related to spoken feedback, divided into the following four categories: a type of feedback (e.g. I like it when my teacher explicitly tells me I made a mistake and gives me the right version of it), the frequency of given feedback (e.g. Teachers should correct students every time they make a mistake when speaking/ writing English), positive attitude towards CF (e.g. Error correction is good for language learning) and negative attitude towards CF (e.g. I usually feel embarrassed when my teacher corrects me in front of the whole class). Each of the mentioned constructs of the survey has three to four questions addressing distinct focus (Calderón, 2013). This part of the survey was used to investigate the students' perception of CF and its association with learners' motivation to speak and write in English as a FL, as well as their EFL achievement.

To fill in the items provided in the questionnaire, the participants were kindly asked to read all the statements carefully and without exception and to choose the preferred number (1-5) which demonstrates their beliefs about the given items. To complete the survey, the participants needed approximately 25 minutes.

Data Analysis

The Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS) version 23.0 was computed for examining the data, and four different statistical procedures were applied. Before testing the hypothesis, the normality test was performed (Byrne, 2010; Chua, 2013; Hair et al., 2010). To determine the extent of the students' motivation to speak and write in English as well as their attitude towards CF in the English language classroom, means and standard deviation were calculated. Then, the Person correlation coefficients were calculated to explore the relationship between the constructs of CF and motivation. One-way MANCOVA was performed in order to determine the impact of gender on writing and speaking motivation with the impact of English study duration being controlled. Finally, standard multiple regression was employed to examine students' motivation and the impact of corrective feedback on students' English language achievement.

RESULTS

Initial Analyses

The initial analysis measured the mean values and standard deviation on all subscales of motivation, i.e. amotivation, external regulation, introjected regulation, identified regulation, intrinsic motivation for knowledge, intrinsic motivation for accomplishment, and intrinsic motivation for stimulation. It also measured the mean values and standard deviation of all variables of corrective feedback, i.e. frequency of given feedback, positive attitude towards corrective feedback, and negative attitudes towards corrective feedback (Table 2).

In terms of speaking motivation subscales, the analysis revealed that students experienced identified regulation as the highest level of motivation with a rather high mean value, followed by intrinsic motivation for accomplishment, introjected regulation, intrinsic motivation for knowledge, external regulation and intrinsic motivation for experiencing stimulation. Rather expectedly, the lowest mean was measured for amotivation. With regards to corrective feedback, the findings related to three categories of the CF scales revealed that participants generally like to be corrected frequently and they were quite positive about it, with positive attitudes being rather high and negative attitudes moderate. Skewness and kurtosis values showed that all the variables are normally distributed since the range of normality of skewness and kurtosis is from -2 to +2 (Garson, 2012; Hair et al., 2010).

Moreover, the mean values and standard deviation of all of subscales of motivation for writing and written corrective feedback were also measured and the data are presented in Table 3.

Table 2

Descriptive Analysis of Motivation Subscales and Categories of CF Scale for Speaking

Measure M SD Skewness Kurtosis

Amotivation 2.58 .79 -.052 -.458

External regulation 3.34 .63 -.150 .343

Introjected regulation 3.67 .66 -1.001 2.045

Identified regulation 3.76 .64 -.545 1.011

Intrinsic motivation for knowledge 3.65 .86 -.400 -.492

Intrinsic motivation for accomplishment 3.72 .77 -.926 1.604

Intrinsic motivation for stimulation 3.30 .89 -.156 -.514

Frequency of given CF 3.15 .78 -.145 .167

Positive attitude towards CF 3.78 .64 -.333 .499

Negative attitude towards CF 2.93 .88 -.228 -.219

Table 3

Descriptive Analysis of Motivation Subscales and Categories of CF Scale for Writing

Measure M SD Skewness Kurtosis

Amotivation 2.60 .81 -.159 -.603

External regulation 3.19 .63 -.122 .185

Introjected regulation 3.55 .68 -.653 1.103

Identified regulation 3.74 .67 -.679 1.233

Intrinsic motivation for knowledge 3.65 .82 -.400 -.395

Intrinsic motivation for accomplishment 3.70 .81 -.924 .911

Intrinsic motivation for stimulation 3.40 .85 -.036 -.964

Frequency of given CF 3.27 .75 -.377 .829

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Positive attitude towards CF 3.70 .67 -.310 -.429

Negative attitude towards CF 2.83 .92 .173 -.289

The findings related to motivation for writing indicated that the participants scored the highest mean on identified regulation, followed by intrinsic motivation for accomplishment, intrinsic motivation for knowledge, introjected regulation, intrinsic motivation for stimulation, and external regulation. The participants again obtained the lowest mean on amotivation. Considering the frequency of receiving feedback, the current study students generally like to be corrected when they make an error in their writing. Overall, they appear to have positive attitude towards error correction and when they are corrected in writing, they feel that they learn more. As for their negative attitude towards correction, the mean value was not rather high. The skewness and kurtosis values again show that all the variables are normally distributed (Hair et al., 2010).

The Relationship between Oral CF and EFL Motivation to Speak

The analysis further measured the relationship between the subscales of oral corrective feedback and motivation for speaking English as a foreign language (Table 4). The interrelationship analyses revealed that the frequency of CF (r = -.16 p = .048) as well as negative attitude (r = -.33 p = .000) towards correction are negatively and significantly correlated with EFL motivation for speaking, while positive attitudes towards CF and motivation for speaking are positively and significantly correlated (r = .19, p =.017). Therefore, those students who have rather negative attitudes towards correction and who are corrected more frequently seem to be less motivated for speaking English, while those students who have positive attitudes towards

CF are more motivated to speak in a foreign language. The correlation between negative attitudes towards CF and positive attitudes towards CF is negative and statistically significant (r = -.20, p = .01). In addition, it is found that positive attitudes towards CF (r = .00, p > .05) and negative attitudes towards CF (r = .15, p > .05) are positively but insignificantly related to the frequency of CF.

The Relationship between Written CF and EFL Motivation to Write

The relationship between the subscales of oral corrective feedback and motivation for writing was also measured (Table 4). The correlation analyses show that writing motivation is positively and significantly correlated only with positive attitudes towards error correction (r = .21 p = .008). It is negatively but statistically insignificantly correlated with the frequency of given feedback (r = -.12, p > .05) and negatively and statistically significantly correlated with negative attitudes towards feedback (r = -.22, p = .004). This indicates that the students who have higher negative attitudes towards CF are less motivated to write. The results also revealed a negative and insignificant correlation between positive attitudes towards CF and its frequency (r = -.11, p > .05), as well as negative and statistically significant correlation between positive and negative attitudes towards corrective feedback (r = -.23, p = .004). The correlation between the frequency of given feedback and negative attitudes towards error correction was positive and statistically significant (r = .17, p = .030).

motivation F (1, 909) = .286, p = .593, n 2 = .002, as well as on writing motivation F (1, 005) = .002, p = .966, n 2 = .000. The differences were also insignificant on the controlling variable for speaking motivation F (7, 619) = 2.400, p = .123, n 2 = .015 as well as for writing motivation F (4, 895) = .000, p = .997, n 2 = .000.

Corrective Feedback as a Language Achievement Predictor

With the intention of determining the accuracy of the frequency of CF, positive attitudes towards CF, and negative attitudes towards CF in speaking and writing in forecasting the learners' accomplishment visible in terms of the English average grade, the standard multiple regression was conducted. The regression analysis showed that error correction scale insignificantly predicted attainment in mastering English as a foreign language R2 = .056, R2 adj. = .019 F (6,153) = 1.768, p = .173. The regression coefficient displayed in Table 5 also demonstrates that higher levels of positive attitudes towards speaking and writing are associated with higher levels of students' EFL achievement despite its insignificance. Likewise, the frequency of CF and negative attitudes towards CF in terms of both speaking and writing do not significantly predict the students' EFL performance but are negatively related to the student's achievement, as seen in Table 5.

DISCUSSION

The Relationship between Gender and EFL Motivation to Speak and Write

A Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was performed to examine the effect of gender on EFL motivation to speak and write (RAI variables), while the impact of the duration of studying English was controlled. The MANCOVA results revealed insignificant differences between female and male participants on the combined depended variable [Wilks' A = .996, F (2, 156) = .324, p = .724, multivariate n 2 = .004]. The results were also insignificant on the covariate duration of studying English [Wilks' A = .948, F (2, 156) = 2.435, p = .091 >.001, multivariate n 2 = .030]. Likewise, differences based on gender were insignificant on speaking

Table 4

Correlations for Speaking/Writing

Descriptive Data

The current paper aimed to explore Bosnian EFL learners' speaking and writing motivation and their stance towards oral and written CF received during English classes. The results showed that these learners in general like to be corrected since the majority of them have positive attitudes towards given feedback, which implies that they are convinced that CF accelerates their learning. Such findings are closely aligned with those reported by Schulz (1996), who found that 90% of the participants hold rather positive attitudes towards correction and grammar teaching, as well as those presented by Hyland (1998) and Ferris and Roberts (2001), who claimed that students expect to be corrected

N12 3 4

1. Speaking motivation (RAI)

2. Frequency of given CF

3. Positive attitudes toward CF

4. Negative attitudes toward CF

160 160 160 160

1/1 -.16*/-.12 .19*/.21** -.33**/-.22**

1/1 .00/-.11 .15/.17*

1/1 -.20*/-.23*

1/1

Table 5

Multiple Regression CF Speaking and Writing

B B TP Bivariate r Partial r

Frequency CF speaking -.095 -.068 -.428 .669 -.116 -.035

Frequency CF writing -.023 -.015 -.098 .922 -.109 -.008

Positive attitudes speaking .082 .048 .350 .727 .092 .028

Positive attitudes writing .012 .007 .052 .958 .101 .004

Negative attitudes speaking -.159 -.128 -.647 .518 -.214 -.052

Negative attitudes writing -.080 -.067 -.339 .735 -.211 -.027

and they perceive CF as beneficial while learning the target language.

The current study participants' answers linked to the frequency of given feedback were quite diverse. Overall, it was shown that the participants like to be frequently corrected because error correction helps them to learn more by transforming a wrong utterance they have produced. These results are in accordance with those reported in the study conducted by Kim and Mathes (2001), who argued that continued feedback is needed when compared to limited feedback, especially if an error occurs more frequently. What is rather interesting is that the participants' negative attitudes towards both oral and written error correction were measured as moderately low, which also corroborates some previous findings suggesting that error correction has a remarkable role in the process of teaching and learning a second or foreign language (see Ellis, 2009;Hattie & Timperley, 2007; Li & Vuono, 2019; Ramsden, 1992).

In terms of motivation to speak and write in a foreign language, the results revealed that it is somehow high though the participants of the present study seem to be more motivated to speak in English, which might be attributed to the fact that writing is frequently cited as the most challenging proficiency target (Lalicic & Dubravac, 2021) as well as to the fact that the majority of language learners wish to develop their speaking skills at a rather fast rate, since "someone's fluency in speaking measures his/her proficiency in that language" (Martin, 2011, p. 237). These findings are consistent with previous research on motivation in the same context. For instance, Becirovic and Brdarevic-Celjo (2018) and Ahmetovic et al. (2020) also indicated that Bosnian students are highly motivated to learn the English language. This can be ascribed to the fact that Bosnia and Herzegovina is a country where the English language is widespread (Brdarevic-Celjo & Dubravac, 2022; Brdarevic-Celjo et al., 2018; Brdarevic-Celjo et al., 2021) and presents an effective means of establishing good communication as well as close cooperation (Brdarevic-Celjo & Asotic, 2017; Becirovic, 2023; Dubravac & Latic, 2019; Dubravac & Skopljak, 2020, Ribo & Dubravac, 2021). Being influenced by the media

as well as by economic and technological advancements, these Bosnian students feel more motivated to put more effort into learning English as a foreign language (Brdarevic et al., 2021; Delic et al., 2018; Dubravac, Brdarevic-Celjo & Becirovic, 2018; Dubravac, Brdarevic-Celjo & Begagic, 2018; Kovacevic et al., 2018).

The Relationship between Oral CF and EFL Motivation to Speak

The first research question sought to identify whether oral CF significantly correlates with motivation for speaking in English. The current study correlation results indicated that positive attitudes towards error correction are significantly correlated with motivation, which means that more positive attitudes towards receiving feedback help develop the participants' proficiency and those who complete the task successfully are expected to continue working hard to improve in the areas where they are less skillful and will be motivated to increase their effort. These results are in line with those in some other studies which also showed a positive and significant correlation between CF and motivation (e.g., DeKeyser, 1993; Guilloteaux & Dornyei, 2008). On the other hand, negative attitudes towards correction correlated negatively and significantly with motivation, which shows that the current study participants who are not well motivated will adopt negative stances towards their tutors' correction and the other way round. The frequency of given feedback also significantly correlated with motivation, but the correlation coefficient was negative. Even though it is generally believed that correction enhances language proficiency, still, teachers should be aware of the fact that frequent error correction in spoken activities disturbs the flow of speech and is the cause of learners' demoralization while performing classroom activities (Lasagabaster & Sierra, 2005).

The Relationship between Written CF and EFL Motivation to Write

To respond to the second research question, the correlation between motivation for writing in English and

three subcategories of written corrective feedback, i.e. frequency of CF, negative and positive attitude towards error corrections, was investigated. In particular, positive attitude towards written CF significantly correlates with the participants' motivation to write, indicating that the current study participants who are well disposed towards written CF have stronger motivation to write. On the contrary, two other subcategories of CF correlated negatively with motivation to write, namely frequency towards given CF non-significantly, and negative attitudes towards CF significantly, which shows that the current study participants who are not well disposed towards CF do not feel motivated particularly if they are corrected frequently. These results are aligned with some other studies (e.g., Bitchener, 2017;Bitchener & Storch, 2016; Ferris, 2003; Leki, 1991), which showed that learners consider feedback valuable and if they do not receive feedback they want, they may lose motivation (Ferris, 2003). Additionally, learners who enjoy learning English, despite having negative attitudes towards correction and constantly being corrected, may find learning challenging and thus become more motivated. Bearing in mind that writing is a difficult (Graham & Harris, 2013; Ogric-Kevric & Dubravac, 2017) and an unattractive task (Boscolo & Gelati, 2013; Hamzic & Becirovic, 2021) involving multiple steps, teachers should nurture and support students by giving meaningful feedback on the areas that need improvement. Hence, the feedback provided by teachers may raise learners' motivation. Though the frequency of given feedback is positively correlated with learners' motivation, constant and extensive correction and taking notice of every mistake the learner makes is needless (Graham et al., 2011). Overall, as one of the major factors influencing students' motivation, feedback needs to be an essential part in acquiring writing skills (Schunk et al., 2014), but teachers should be cautious while employing it as in some research studies no advantages of WFC were observed (e.g. Polio et al., 1998).

The Relationship between Gender and EFL Motivation to Speak and Write

The third question researched the impact of gender on the participants' motivation for speaking and writing in English when the duration of studying English is controlled. Overall, the current findings did not find that gender had a significant impact on the participants' writing and speaking motivation as suggested by previous research (Becirovic et al., 2022; Dornyei & Csizer, 2005; Harthy, 2017; Xiong, 2010). Still, the current findings are in accordance with the findings of some other studies (Akram & Ghani, 2013), in which no gender-based differences were measured in terms of motivation. Despite the non-existent gender differences in the current study, the male participants had an insignificantly higher level of speaking motivation, while the female participants were more motivated in terms of writing.

Corrective Feedback as Language Achievement Predictor

The fourth research question asked whether oral and written CF in English significantly predict the participants' language achievement. In the current study, neither oral nor written CF significantly predicted the participants' EFL achievement. Taking into consideration different components of oral as well as written corrective feedback (frequency of given feedback, as well as negative and positive attitude towards error correction), the current study showed that positive attitude is insignificantly and positively associated with the learners' attainment, which indicates that the current study participants who are well disposed towards error correction might be more successful in learning the target language since they believe that CF facilitates their progress in developing greater EFL proficiency. This finding is in accordance with the past research discoveries which demonstrated correction effectiveness in improving students' achievement (Bitchener et al., 2005; Choi, 2013; Hyland, 1998; Lee, 2004; Sheen, 2007). The frequency of given feedback and negative attitude towards error correction are insignificantly but negatively related to the participants' attainment, which suggests that these participants find continuous correction very annoying, distracting and ineffective and some of them would even stop participating in the classroom activities to avoid being frequently corrected. Thus, too much feedback can hinder students' achievement as they might feel controlled, which results in putting less effort into learning activities. Therefore, as it was confirmed previously by the great majority of studies, correction should take place in EFL classrooms, since it improves students' accuracy (see Kepner, 1991; Sheppard, 1992), but teachers should be cautious as to how frequently they employ it especially during speaking activities. To sum up, it seems that learners with positive attitude towards CF will benefit more from correction and will, thus, succeed in increasing their language proficiency than those with negative attitudes as correction can impact the scope of their engagement in learning activities (Sheen, 2007).

CONCLUSION

Error making is an unavoidable aspect of language learning, which should not be hindered by error correction. In general, students tend to believe that they should receive correction when they make a mistake, and that teachers are tasked with the responsibility of providing learners with correct information. Even though implementing error correction properly can be challenging, it is crucial that teachers are knowledgeable of learners' attitudes towards given feedback so that they can use correction effectively while addressing students' errors and not decrease their motivation.

Therefore, the aim of the current study was to investigate students' motivation to speak and write in English and its correlation with their attitude towards error correction and frequency of given feedback based on their gender, achievement, and duration of EFL learning. On the one hand, learners in Bosnia have different attitudes towards provided feedback and its frequency, so the students with positive attitudes towards CF and frequency of given feedback feel more motivated to continue improving in the target language in comparison with the students who are not so well disposed towards the received feedback. Generally, the participants of the current study react positively to error feedback, which means that they take it seriously, pay attention to it and appreciate it highly. Thus, they appear to believe in the benefits of CF, and they agree that error correction is necessary to help them see the difference between a wrong utterance and the right version of it. Furthermore, the results suggest that those students who are more negatively oriented towards CF might feel more embarrassed, which could eventually diminish their self-confidence and hinder their language learning achievement.

Led by these research results, teachers can apply different strategies of error correction with the aim of motivating learners in an attempt to obtain significant results in acquiring English as a foreign language. For instance, while doing oral activities, teachers can provide feedback in various ways: teachers may record learners while speaking and at the end of the activity, they might firstly ask students whether they are aware of their own errors or write both the correct and incorrect versions (the one that students have used) on the board and ask students to choose the right one. When doing this, teachers should not point to the person who made the mistake in order to avoid increasing students' sense of insecurity. To achieve this, teachers should concentrate on those mistakes made by more than one person. If teachers want to focus on individual students' mistakes, they can write individual notes to students about the mistakes they have made, with explanations or suggestions as to where to find more information about those mistakes. On the other hand, giving feedback on the written language production will depend on the characteristics of a writing task. For example, in workbook exercises, teachers can mark the responses as either correct or incorrect, while in more creative or communicative writing, such as reports, essays, stories, and so on, teachers need to approach the task with caution demonstrating their curiosity in the content of the learners' assignment rather than focusing on grammar. This could be done by responding to students' work in a form of written feedback emphasizing strong and weak sides of the composition rather than just assessing the work by a numerical grade. Although all of these methods are time-consuming, they are very useful and more beneficial than the other methods used in error correction. Additionally, in writing activities teachers can use codes or symbols and put them in the body of writing. This makes correction more

helpful and less threatening than responding comments, and they are very useful while referring to issues such as spelling, verb tenses, word order, etc. However, students' interest in improving their language skills plays an important part in the process of learning as it can lead to a positive attitude towards teachers' correction and towards learning English in general. Taking this into account, teachers should find appropriate measures to arouse students' curiosity and develop their interest in EFL learning.

Limitations

The present study has some limitations. Firstly, only students and not teachers were included in the research, although teachers' attitudes towards error correction play a significant role in error correction and in L2 motivation. Consequently, as the present study did not include teachers as participants, the obtained results might not be fully representative of the reality in this foreign language context especially in terms of teachers' beliefs. Additionally, as the study did not include any classroom observation, learners' views about error correction as well as the frequency of given feedback might have been purely subjective and the results could not have been confirmed through a more objective measurement method. Thus, longitudinal observation as well as a mixed method design (both quantitative and qualitative) might lead to more concrete results about the relationship between EFL motivation and CF.

DECLARATION OF COMPETITING INTEREST

None declared.

AUTHORS' CONTRIBUTION

Emnijeta Ahmetovic: Conceptualization;Funding acquisition;Investigation;Methodology;Project administration; Resources; Supervision; Validation; Writing-review and editing.

Senad Becirovic: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Resources; Software; Supervision; Validation.

Vildana Dubravac: Conceptualization;Data curation; Investigation;Methodology;Project administration; Supervision; Validation; Visualization; Writing-original draft.

Amna Brdarevic Celjo: Conceptualization; Data curation; Investigation;Methodology;Project administration; Supervision;Validation;Writing-original draft;Writing-review and editing.

REFERENCES

Ahmetovic, E., Becirovic, S., & Dubravac, V. (2020). Motivation, anxiety and students' performance. European Journal of Contemporary Education, 9(2), 271-289. https://doi.Org/10.13187/ejced.2020.2.271

Al-Darei, I. S. & Ahmed, A. M. (2022). The effect of feedback type in the e-learning environment on students' achievement and motivation. Journal of Educational Technology & Online Learning, 5(3), 694-705. http://doi.org/10.31681/jetol.1111527

Al-Rubai'ey, F., & Nassaji, H. (2013). Direct and indirect metalinguistic feedback: A matter of suitability rather than superiority. In M. Mahmoud & R. Al Mahrooqi (Eds.), TEFL in the Arab World (pp. 28-43). Sultan Qaboos University Press. https://doi. org/10.1558/isla.37949

Akram, M., & Ghani, M. (2013). Gender and language learning motivation. Academic Research International, 4(2), 536-540.

Ashwell, T. (2000). Patterns of teacher response to student writing in a multi-draft composition classroom: Is content

feedback followed by form feedback the best method? Journal of Second Language Writing, 9(3), 227-257. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.03.484.

Becirovic, S. (2023). Digital pedagogy: The use of digital technologies in contemporary education. Springer.

Becirovic, S., & Brdarevic-Celjo, A., (2018). Exploring and assessing cross-cultural sensitivity in Bosnian tertiary education: Is there a real promise of harmonious coexistence? European Journal of Contemporary Education, 7(2), 244-256. https://doi. org/10.13187/ejced.2018.2.244

Becirovic, S., Dubravac, V., & Brdarevic-Celjo, A. (2022). Cooperative learning as a pathway to strengthening motivation and improving achievement in an EFL classroom. SAGE Open, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/21582440221078016

Bitchener, J., Young, S., & Cameron, D. (2005). The effect of different types of corrective feedback on ESL student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14(3), 191-205. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjslw.2005.08.001

Bitchener, J. (2008). Evidence in support of written corrective feedback. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17(2), 102-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjslw.2007.11.004

Bitchener, J., & Storch, N. (2016). Written corrective feedback for L2 development. Multilingual Matters. https://doi. org/10.21832/9781783095056

Bitchener, J. (2017). Why some L2 learners fail to benefit from written corrective feedback. In H. Nassaji & E. Kartchava (Eds.), Corrective feedback in second language teaching and learning (pp. 129-40). Routledge. https://doi. org/10.4324/9781315621432-10

Boscolo, P., & Gelati, C. (2013). Best practices in promoting motivation for writing. In S. Graham, C.A. MacArthur, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Best practices in writing instruction (2nd ed., pp. 202- 222). The Guilford Press.

Brdarevic-Celjo, A., & Asotic, M. (2017). The influence of social context, grade level and gender on the use of language learning strategies in primary schools. Imperial Journal of Interdisciplinary Research, 3(12), 7-14.

Brdarevic-Celjo, A., Becirovic, S., & Burekovic, M. (2018). The use of imaginative conditional clauses by Bosnian university-level English-majoring students. The Journal of Linguistic and Intercultural Education, 11(2), 25-40. https://doi.org/10.29302/ jolie.2018.11.2.2

Brdarevic-Celjo, A., Ahmetovic, E., & Bajic, E. (2021). Variations in attitudes towards codeswitching and codeswitching frequency among multilingual speakers. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development. Advance online publication. https://doi. org/10.1080/01434632.2021.1983580

Brdarevic-Celjo, A., Becirovic, S., & Dubravac, V. (2021). An examination of perceived reading strategy use among university level students. European Journal of Contemporary Education, 10(3), 595-608. https://doi.org/10.13187/ejced.20213.595

Brdarevic-Celjo, A., & Dubravac, V. (2022). Engleski u BiH: Izmedu ugladenog i lezernog [English in B&H: Posh or casual?]. Multidisciplinary Academic Publishing.

Brown, D. (2016). The type and linguistic foci of oral corrective feedback in the L2 classroom: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching Research, 20(4), 436-458. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168814563200

Bruton, A. (2010). Another reply to Truscott on error correction: Improved situated designs over statistics. System, 38(3), 491498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. system.2010.07.001.

Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications and programming (2nd ed.). Routledge.

Chandler, J. (2009). Response to Truscott. Journal of Second Language Writing, 18(1), 57-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jslw.2008.09.002

Calderón, N. (2013). EFL learner and teacher perspectives on corrective feedback and their effect on second language learning motivation [Unpublished Master's thesis]. McGill University.

Chen, S., Nassaji, H., & Liu, Q. (2016). EFL learners' perceptions and preferences of written corrective feedback: A case study of university students from Mainland China. Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education, 1(5), 1-17. https:// doi.org/10.1186/s40862-016-0010-y

Chen, S., & Nassaji, H. (2018). Focus on form and corrective feedback at the University of Victoria. Language Teaching, 51(2), 278-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S026144481800006X

Choi, S. (2013). The effects of written corrective feedback on second language writing focused on the English writing system [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. ProQuest Digital Dissertations.

Chua, Y. P. (2013). Mastering research statistics. McGraw-Hill Education.

DeKeyser, R.M. (1993). The effect of error correction on L2 grammar knowledge and oral proficiency. The Modern Language Journal, 77(4), 501-514. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1540-4781.1993.TB01999.X

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237-288). University of Nebraska Press.

Delic, H., Becirovic, S., & Brdarevic Celjo, A. (2018). Effects of grade level and gender on foreign language learning process in Bosnian high schools. International Journal of Educational Policy Research and Review, 5(6), 83-89. https://doi.org/10.15739/ IJEPRR.18.010

Dervic, M., & Becirovic, S. (2019). Native and non-native EFL teachers dichotomy: Terminological, competitiveness and employment discrimination. Journal of Language and Education, 5(3), 114-127. https://doi.org/10.17323/jle.2019.9746

Doughty C., & Long M. (2003). Optimal psycholinguistic environments for distance foreign language learning. Language Learning & Technology, 7(3), 50-80. http://dx.doi.org/10125/25214

Doughty, C. J., & Williams, J. (1998). Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition. Cambridge University Press.

Dornyei, Z. (1994). Motivation and motivating in the foreign language classroom. The Modern LanguageJournal, 78(3), 273-284. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1994.tb02042.x.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Dornyei, Z. (2000). Motivation in action: Towards a process-oriented conceptualization of student motivation. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(4), 519-538. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709900158281

Dornyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Routledge.

Dornyei, Z., & Csizer, K. (1998). Ten commandments for motivating language learners: Results of an empirical study. Language Teaching Research, 2(3), 203-229. https://doi.org/10.1191/136216898668159830

Dornyei, Z., & Ushioda, E. (2011). Teaching and Researching Motivation (2nd ed.). Pearson.

Dubravac, V., Brdarevic-Celjo, A., & Becirovic, S. (2018). The English of Bosnia and Herzegovina. World Englishes, 37(4), 635652. https://doi.org/10.1111/weng.12347

Dubravac, V., Brdarevic-Celjo, A., & Begagic, M. (2018). The presence of the British and American English variety among the first-year students at the University of Zenica. In A. Arnaut (Ed.), Saznanje, Zbornikradova 7. medunarodnog naucno -strucnog skupa: Obrazovanje, jezik, kultura: tendencije i izazovi [Cognition, Proceedings of the 7th International Scientific and Expert Meeting: Eduation, Language, Culture: Tendencies and Challenges] (pp. 518-528). University of Zenica.

Dubravac, V., & Latic, E. (2019). The plasticity of students' language learning beliefs: Interplay of gender, grade and educational level. Journal of Language and Education Education, 5(4), 36-53. https://doi.org/10.17323/jle.2019.9732

Dubravac, V., & Skopljak, N. (2020). Foreign and multilingual language play on social sites as an identity marker. Journal of Multicultural Discourses, 15(1), 61-79. https://doi.org/10.1080/17447143.2019.1701678.

Ellis R., (2009). Corrective feedback and teacher development. L2 Journal, 1(1), 3-18. https://doi.org/10.5070/l2.v1i1.9054

Ellis, R. (2010). A framework for investigating oral and written corrective feedback. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 335-349. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263109990544

Ellis, R., Sheen, Y., Murakami, M., & Takashima, H. (2008). The effects of focused and unfocused written corrective feedback in an English as a foreign language context. System, 36(3), 353-371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2008.02.001

Evans, N. W., Hartshorn, K. J., & Tuioti, E. A. (2010). Written corrective feedback: Practitioners' perspectives. InternationalJournal of English Studies, 10(2), 47-77. https://doi.org/10.6018/ijes.10.2.119191

Ferris, D. R. (2003). Response to student writing: Implications for second language students. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Ferris, D. (2006). Does error feedback help student writers? New evidence on the short- and long-term effects of written error correction. In K. Hyland & F. Hyland (Eds.), Feedback in second language writing: Contexts and issues (pp. 81-104). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CB09781139524742.007

Ferris, D. (2010). Second language writing research and written corrective feedback in SLA. Intersections and practical applications. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(2), 181-201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263109990490

Ferris, D., & Roberts, B. (2001). Error feedback in L2 writing classes. How explicit does it need to be? Journal of Second Language Writing, 10(3), 161-184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1060-3743(01)00039-X

Flynn, S. (1996). Nature of development in L2 acquisition and implications for theories of language acquisition in general. In S. Flynn & W. O'Neill (Eds), Linguistic theory in second language acquisition (pp. 277-294). Kluwer. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0272263100009050

Gardner, R. (2011). Language learning motivation: The student, the teacher and the researcher. Foreign Language Education, 6(1), 1-18.

Gardner, R. C., & MacIntyre, P. D. (1993). On the measurement of affective variables in second language learning. Language Learning, 43(2), 157-194. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1992.tb00714.x

Garson, G. D. (2012). Testing statistical assumptions. Statistical Associates Publishing.

Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2001). Second Language Acquisition, an Introductory Course, (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gass, S. (2003). Input and interaction. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 104-129). Blackwell Publishing Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1111/b.9781405132817.2005.00001.x

Gooch, R., Saito, K., & Lyster, R. (2016). Effects of recasts and prompts on L2 pronunciationdevelopment: Teaching English /j/ to Korean adult EFL learners. System, 60, 117-127. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2016.06.007

Graham, S., Harris, K., & Hebert, M.A. (2011). Informing writing: The benefits of formative assessment. A Carnegie Corporation Time to Act report. Alliance for Excellent Education.

Graham, S., & Harris, K.R. (2013). Designing an effective writing program. In S. Graham, C.A. MacArthur, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Best practices in writing instruction (pp. 3-25). The Guilford Press.

Guilloteaux, M.J., & Dornyei, Z. (2008). Motivating language learners: A classroom-oriented investigation of the effects of motivational strategies on student motivation. TESOL Quarterly, 42, 55-77. https://doi.org/10.1002/j. 1545-7249.2008. tb00207.x

Ha, X. V., Nguyen, L. T., & Hung, B. P. (2021). Oral corrective feedback in English as a foreign language classrooms: A teaching and learning perspective. Heliyon, 7(7), e07550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e07550

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis: A global perspective. Pearson Education International.

Hamidun, H., Hizvari, Sh., & Othman, N. F. (2012). Enhancing student's motivation by providing feedback on writing: the case of international students from Thailand. International Journal of Social Science and Humanity, 2(6), 591-594. https://doi. org/10.7763/IJSSH.2012.V2.179

Hamzic, U., & Becirovic, S. (2021). Twice-exceptional, half-noticed: The recognition issues of gifted students with learning disabilities. MAP Social Sciences, 1(1), 13-22. https://doi.org/10.53880/2744-2454.2021.1.1.13

Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77(1), 81-112. https://doi. org/10.3102/003465430298487

Hartshorn, K. J., Evans, N. W., Merrill, P. F., Sudweeks, R. R., Strong-Krause, D., & Anderson, N. J. (2010). Effects of dynamic corrective feedback on ESL writing accuracy. TESOLQuarterly, 44(1), 84-109. https://doi.org/10.5054/tq.2010.213781

Hedgcock, J., & Lefkowitz, N. (1996). Some input on input: Two analyses of student response to expert feedback in L2 writing. The Modern Language Journal, 80(3), 287-308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1996.tb01612.x

Hyland, K. (1998). Boosting, hedging and the negotiation of academic knowledge. TEXT, 18(3), 349-382. https://doi.org/10.1515/ text.1.1998.18.3.349

Hyland, K. (2003). Second language writing. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511667251

Hyland, K., & Hyland, F. (2006). Feedback on second language students' writing (vol. 39). Cambridge University Press. https://doi. org/10.1017/S0261444806003399

Jean, G., & Simard, D. (2011). Grammar learning in English and French L2: Students' and teachers' beliefs and perceptions. Foreign Language Annals, 44(4), 465-492. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2011.01143.x

Kalaja, P., & Barcelos, A. M. F. (Eds.). (2003). Beliefs about SLA: New research approaches. Kluwer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-4751-0

Kang, E., & Han, Z. (2015). The efficacy of written corrective feedback in improving L2 written accuracy: A meta-analysis. Modern Language Journal, 9(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12189

Karim, K., & Nassaji, H. (2018). The revision and transfer effects of direct and indirect comprehensive corrective feedback on English-as-a-second-language (ESL) students' writing. Language Teaching Research, 3(1), 29-69. https://doi. org/10.1177/1362168818802469

Karim, K., & Nassaji, H. (2019). The effects of written corrective feedback: a critical synthesis of past and present research. Instructed Second Language Acquisition, 3(1), 28-52. https://doi.org/10.1558/isla.37949

Kepner, C. G. (1991). An experiment in the relationship of types of written feedback to the development of second-language writing skills. The Modern Language Journal, 75(3), 305-313. https://doi.org/10.2307/328724

Kim, H., & Mathes, G. (2001). Explicit vs. implicit corrective feedback. The Korea TESOLJournal, 4(1), 1-15.

Kovacevic, F., Brdarevic-Celjo, A., & Becirovic, S. (2018). Opportunities and challenges facing Bosnian high-school EFL learners. European Researcher. Series A, 9(4), 298-306. https://doi.org/10.13187/er.2018.4298

Krashen, S. (1982). Newmark's «Ignorance Hypothesis» and current second language acquisition theory [Unpublished manuscript].

Lalicic, A., & Dubravac, V. (2021). The role of reading in English language classrooms. MAP Social Sciences, 1(1), 23-36. https:// doi.org/10.53880/2744-2454.2021.1.1.23

Lasagabaster, D., & Sierra, J. M. (2005). What do students think about the pros and cons of having a native speaker teacher? In E. Llurda (Ed.), Non-Native language teachers (pp. 217-241. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-24565-0_12

Lee, I. (2004). Error correction in L2 secondary writing classrooms: The case of Hong Kong. Journal of Second Language Writing 13(4), 285-312. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjslw.2004.08.001

Lee, E. J. (2013). Corrective feedback preferences and learner repair among advanced ESL students. System, 41(2), 217-230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2013.01.022

Leki, I. (1991). The preferences of ESL students for error correction in college level writing classes. Foreign Language Annals, 24, 203-218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.1991.tb00464.x

Li, S. (2010). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 309-365. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00561.x

Li, S., & Vuono, A. (2019). Twenty-five years of research on oral and written corrective feedback. System, 84, 93-109. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.system.2019.05.006

Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413-468). Academic Press.

Lopez, M. B., Steendam, E. V., Speelman, D., & Buyse, K. (2018) The differential effects of comprehensive feedback forms in the second language writing class. Language Learning, 68(3), 813-850. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12295

Lyster, R., Lightbown, P. M., & Spada, N. (1999). A response to Truscott's 'What's wrong with oral grammar correction'. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 55(4), 457-467. https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.55.4457

Lyster, R., & Saito, K. (2010). Oral feedback in classroom SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(2), 265-302. https://doi. org/10.1017/S0272263109990520

Lyster R., Saito K., & Sato M. (2013). Oral corrective feedback in second language classrooms. Language Teaching, 46(1), 1-40. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444812000365

Masic, A., Edda P. E., & Becirovic, S. (2020). The Relationship between Learning Styles, GPA, School Level and Gender. European Researcher. Series A, 11(1), 51-60. https://doi.org/10.13187/er.2020.1.51

Nassaji, H. (2015). The interactional feedback dimension in instructed second language learning: Linking theory, research, and practice. Bloomsbury.

Nassaji, H. (2016). Interactional feedback in second language teaching and learning: A synthesis and analysis of current research. Language Teaching Research, 20(4), 535-562. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816644940

Nassaji, H. (2017). The effectiveness of extensive versus intensive recasts for learning L2 grammar. The Modern Language Journal, 101(2), 353-368. https://doi.org/10.1111/ modl.12387

Nassaji, H., & Kartchava, E. (2020). Corrective feedback and good language teachers. In C. Griffiths & Z. Tajeddin (Eds.), Lessons from good language teachers (pp. 151-163). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108774390.015

Ogric-Kevric S., & Dubravac V. (2017). Students' and teachers' perceptions on skills acquisition and error correction treatment in Bosnian EFL context. Imperial Journal of Interdisciplinary Research, 3(9), 187-194.

Ortega, L. (2009). Understanding second language acquisition. Hodder Education.

Papi M., Rios A., Pelt H., & Ozdemir E. (2019). Feedback-seeking behavior in language learning: Basic components and motivational antecedents. The Modern Language Journal, 103(1), 205-226. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12538

Patra, I., Alazemi, A. F., Al-Jamal, D., & Gheisari, A. (2022). The effectiveness of teachers' written and verbal corrective feedback (CF) during formative assessment (FA) on male language learners' academic anxiety (AA), academic performance (AP), and attitude toward learning (ATL). Language Testing in Asia. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40468-022-00169-2

Polio, C., Fleck, C., & Leder, N. (1998). 'If I only had more time': ESL learners' changes in linguistic accuracy on essay revisions. Journal of Second Language Writing, 7, 43-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1060-3743(98)90005-4

Polz, E., & Becirovic, S. (2022). Competency based teaching and learning. Multidisciplinary Academic Publishing.

Plonsky, L., & Brown, D. (2015). Domain definition and search techniques in meta-analyses of L2 research (Or why 18 metanalyses of feedback have different results). Second Language Research, 31(2), 267-278. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658314536436

Ramsden, P. (1992). Learning to teach in higher education. learning to teach in higher education. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.1 080/03075079312331382498

Ribo, R., & Vildana D. (2021). The influence of the English language on BCS with the focus on the business register. MAP Education and Humanities, 1(1), 20-27. https://doi.org/10.53880/2744-2373.2021.1.1.20

Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, I. (1986). Salience of feedback on error and its effect on EFL writing quality. TESOL Quarterly, 20(1), 83-93. https://doi.org/10.2307/3586390

Russell, J., & Spada, N. (2006). The effectiveness of corrective feedback for second language acquisition: A meta-analysis of the research. In J. Norris & L. Ortega (Eds.), Synthesizing research on language learning and teaching (pp. 131-164). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.13.09val

Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(5), 749-761. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.5749

Saito, K. (2021). Effects of corrective feedback on second language pronunciation development. In H. Nassaji & E. Kartchava (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of corrective feedback in language learning and teaching (pp. 407-428). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108589789.020

Schachter, J. (1991). Corrective feedback in historical perspective. Second Language Research, 7(2), 89-102. https://doi. org/10.1177/026765839100202

Schulz, R. A. (1996). Focus on form in the foreign language classroom: Students' and teachers' views on error correction and the role of grammar. Foreign Language Annals, 29(3), 343- 364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.1996.tb01247.x

Schulz, R. A. (2001). Cultural differences in student and teacher perceptions concerning the role of grammar teaching and corrective feedback: USA-Colombia. The Modern Language Journal, 85(2), 244-258. https://doi.org/10.1111/0026-7902.00107

Schunk, D. H., Meece, J. R., & Pintrich, P. R. (2014). Motivation in education: Theory, research and applications (4th ed.). Pearson.

Sheen, Y. (2007). The effect of focused written corrective feedback and language aptitude on ESL learners' acquisition of articles. TESOL Quarterly, 41(2), 255 -283. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1545-7249.2007.tb00059.x

Sheppard, K. (1992). Two feedback types: Do they make a difference? RELC Journal, 23(1), 103-110. https://doi. org/10.1177/003368829202300107

Truscott, J. (1999). The case for "the case for grammar correction in L2 writing classes": A response to Ferris. Journal of Second Language Writing, 8(2), 111- 122. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1060-3743(99)80124-6

Unlu, A (2016). Adjusting potentially confounded scoring protocols for motivation aggregation in organismic integration theory: An exemplification with the relative autonomy or self-determination index. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1-4. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00272

Unlu, A. (2019). Self-determination motivation theory in R: The software package SDT. Archives of Psychology, 3(9). https:// archivesofpsychology.org/index.php/aop/article/view/131.

Wang, W. & Li, S. (2020). Corrective feedback and learner up take in American ESL and Chinese EFL classrooms: A comparative study. Language, Culture, and Curriculum, 34(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1080/07908318.2020.1767124.

Yang, Y., & Lyster, R. (2010). Effects of form-focused practice and feedback on Chinese EFL learners' acquisition of regular and irregular past-tense forms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(2), 235-263. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0272263109990519

Yaman, A., & Becirovic, S. (2016). Learning English and media literacy. ImperialJournal of Interdisciplinary Research, 2(6), 660-663.

Yoshida, R. (2008). Teachers' choice and learners' preference of corrective feedback types. Language Awareness, 17(1), 78-93. https://doi.org/10.2167/la429.0

Zamel, V. (1985). Responding to student writing. TESOL Quarterly, 19(1), 79-101. https://doi.org/10.2307/3586773

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.