Научная статья на тему 'EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN CARRIERS OF DIFFERENT СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2 AND HTR2A GENOTYPES'

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN CARRIERS OF DIFFERENT СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2 AND HTR2A GENOTYPES Текст научной статьи по специальности «Фундаментальная медицина»

CC BY
34
6
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Журнал
Psychology in Russia: State of the art
WOS
Scopus
ВАК
Ключевые слова
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE / GENETICS / СОМТ / BDNF / DRD2 / HTR2A

Аннотация научной статьи по фундаментальной медицине, автор научной работы — Vorobyeva Elena V., Kovsh Ekaterina M., Kosonogov Vladimir V.

Background. Emotional intelligence is the ability to quickly and correctly recognize the emotional expressions of other people and to express and manage one’s own emotions. It contributes to the success of a person in activities related to communication and interaction with people. Emotional intelligence has been studied largely in the context of organizational and education psychology, but less is known about the influence of genetics on it. Objective. We aim to study emotional intelligence in carriers of different СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2, and HTR2A genotypes. Design. We used three methods to measure emotional intelligence. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test is a set of tasks with forced choice and frequency-based correct responses. We also applied two self-report questionnaires by Lyusin and Hall. We recruited 280 participants who took part in all three measures. We also identified their genotypes of the СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2, and HTR2A genes. Results. Carriers of the Val/Met genotype of the COMT gene, A/A genotype of the HTR2A gene and C/C genotype of the DRD2 gene showed the highest level of emotional intelligence, while no differences were found between carriers of the BDNF genotypes. These data were obtained by using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Self-report scores of emotional intelligence did not differ between carriers of different genotypes across all four of the genes in question. Conclusion. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test scores were differed for carriers of some genotypes, whereas self-reported emotional intelligence scores did not differ between according to genotype.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Похожие темы научных работ по фундаментальной медицине , автор научной работы — Vorobyeva Elena V., Kovsh Ekaterina M., Kosonogov Vladimir V.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN CARRIERS OF DIFFERENT СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2 AND HTR2A GENOTYPES»

Psychology in Russia: State of the Art Volume 15, Issue 2, 2022

Russian /-¿i^ I Lomonosov Psychological i№ A Moscow State Society ^SW |y|y|| University

PSYCHOGENETICS

Emotional Intelligence in Carriers of Different СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2 and HTR2A Genotypes

Elena V. Vorobyevaa*, Ekaterina M. Kovsh a, Vladimir V. Kosonogovb

a Southern Federal University, Rostov-on-Don, Russia b HSE University, Moscow, Russia

""Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Background. Emotional intelligence is the ability to quickly and correctly recognize the emotional expressions of other people and to express and manage one's own emotions. It contributes to the success of a person in activities related to communication and interaction with people. Emotional intelligence has been studied largely in the context of organizational and education psychology, but less is known about the influence of genetics on it.

Objective. We aim to study emotional intelligence in carriers of different COMT, BDNF, DRD2, and HTR2A genotypes.

Design. We used three methods to measure emotional intelligence. Mayer-Sal-ovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test is a set of tasks with forced choice and frequency-based correct responses. We also applied two self-report questionnaires by Lyusin and Hall. We recruited 280 participants who took part in all three measures. We also identified their genotypes of the COMT, BDNF, DRD2, and HTR2A genes.

Results. Carriers of the Val/Met genotype of the COMT gene, A/A genotype of the HTR2A gene and C/C genotype of the DRD2 gene showed the highest level of emotional intelligence, while no differences were found between carriers of the BDNF genotypes. These data were obtained by using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Self-report scores of emotional intelligence did not differ between carriers of different genotypes across all four of the genes in question.

Conclusion. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test scores were differed for carriers of some genotypes, whereas self-reported emotional intelligence scores did not differ between according to genotype.

Keywords:

Emotional

intelligence,

genetics,

COMT,

BDNF,

DRD2,

HTR2A

ISSN 2074-6857 (Print) / ISSN 2307-2202 (Online) © Lomonosov Moscow State University, 2022 © Russian Psychological Society, 2022 http://psychologyinrussia.com

The journal content is licensed with CC BY-NC "Attribution-NonCommercial" Creative Commons license.

84 Vorobyeva, E.V., Kovsh, E.M., Kosonogov, V.V. Introduction

Emotional intelligence is the ability to quickly and correctly recognize the emotional expressions of other people and to express and manage one's own emotions. Emotional intelligence contributes to the success of a person in activities related to communication and interaction with people (Mayer et al., 2004; Lanthem, 2021). Emotional intelligence has been shown to correlate with a general factor of personality, which indicates a high content similarity of these constructs (Anglim et al., 2020). The level of emotional intelligence is an important factor in social effectiveness, decision-making, and prevention of various risks (Pavlova et al., 2013; Cha et al., 2013; Hosseini et al., 2013; van der Linden et al., 2017; Kundi et al., 2021). Emotional intelligence is considered a resource for human development and well-being. There is a difference in emotional intelligence between men and women. That is, emotional intelligence seems to be higher in females (Sergienko et al., 2020; Fernández et al., 2020).

The concept of emotional intelligence finds its origin in Thorndike's theory of social intelligence. This concept involved the ability to understand, manage, and interact wisely with other people. In the latter part of the 20th century, within the framework of Gardner's ideas about interpersonal intelligence, was formed the idea that emotional intelligence pertains to the ability of an individual to understand the intentions, motivations and desires of both himself and others (quoted by Smith et al., 2018). In modern studies, researchers attempt to study emotional intelligence, among other things, within the context of theory of mind by choosing certain categories of respondents for research (Ferguson et al., 2010; Caldu et al., 2019; Andri-ushchenko et al., 2020).

Two aspects of the ability to recognize emotions can be distinguished: the accuracy of the differentiation between basic emotions and the sensitivity to their intensity. The main ability model of emotional intelligence by Mayer, Salovey and Caruso includes 4 groups of cognitive abilities: identifying emotions, using emotions, understanding emotions, and managing emotions (Mayer et al., 2004; Lyusin et al., 2016; Israelashvili et al., 2019; Pankratova, 2019).

Nowadays, many researchers also focus on the experimental study of brain mechanisms for emotion recognition and the presentation of emotional expressions, using methods for registering evoked brain potentials and other psychophysiologi-cal variables (Raz et al., 2013; Barbey et al., 2014; Lyusin et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2017; Barabanschikov et al., 2019; Alekseeva et al., 2019). As a practical implication, some studies demonstrate that emotional intelligence can be trained (enhanced) by psychological methods (Hodzic, et al., 2018). Like many other psychological traits, emotional intelligence is determined on one hand by genetic influences, and on the other by environmental influences. These include belonging to a particular ethnic culture, which regulates certain features of emotional expression. In addition, there are the effects of genotype-environment interaction and genotype-environment covariance on emotional expression (Pankratova et al., 2016; Dewi et al., 2018; Katrushova et al., 2019). Genes associated with emotional intelligence are involved in the serotoner-gic and dopaminergic neurotransmitter systems, and affect neuroplasticity (Kim et al., 2011; Takeuchi et al., 2015; Kosonogov et al., 2019; Ermakov et al., 2019; Brown et al., 2020). The catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene is associated with the

activity of the striatum and prefrontal regions. This gene is simultaneously associated with aggression, motivation, and recognition of negative emotions. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene is associated with memory disorders, depression, moral emotions, features of perception of affective visual scenes, regulation of emotions, and stress resistance. The dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene is associated with emotional stability, control over emotions and activation of limbic structures, as well as with successful emotion recognition. The serotonin receptor type II (HTR2A) gene is associated with depression, social introversion, and anxiety (Pecina et al., 2013; Gohier et al., 2014; Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., 2015; Alfimova et al., 2019; Lischke et al., 2019; Caldu et al., 2019; Vorobyeva et al., 2020; Redlich et al., 2020; Quan et al., 2021; Tian et al., 2021).

Association of genes and recognition of emotions, emotion expressions and emotion management are often considered by studies in which the participants are affected by disorders like schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder, but not by the study of healthy individuals (Gadow et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2020; Qin et al., 2020; Halicka-Maslowska et al., 2021).

We hypothesized that emotional intelligence can be influenced by genetic factors. It can be associated with dominant or recessive alleles of the COMT, BDNF, DRD2 and HTR2A genes. The aim of this work is to compare emotional intelligence in mentally healthy carriers of the COMT, BDNF, DRD2 and HTR2A genotypes.

Methods Participants

A sample of 280 volunteers (65.6% female; mean age = 19.7; SD = 2.9) participated in the study. The participants lived in the south of Russia: in Rostov-on-Don and Chal-tyr (one of its rural localities), in Nalchik (Republic of Kabardino-Balkaria), and in Karachaevsk (Republic of Karachay-Cherkessia). All participants agreed to voluntarily participate in the study. They were all right-handed with normal (or corrected to normal) vision. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki.

Procedure

We used three methods to measure emotional intelligence: the Mayer-Salovey-Caru-so Emotional Intelligence Test and two self-report questionnaires by Lyusin and Hall. The Russian version (Sergienko & Vetrova, 2010) of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT; Mayer et al., 2003) consists of 141 items that reflect four branches: perceiving emotions, facilitating thought, understanding emotions, and managing emotions. All the items represent a task with multiple choice answers. Unlike with self-report questionnaires or cognitive evaluation, we applied the sample frequency procedure to analyse the responses. Thus, correct responses were not predetermined, but based on the frequency of each option in the studied sample. For each item, each option obtained a score correspondent to its frequency in the given sample of participants. In other words, if a participant selected the most frequent option in an item, they received a greater score of emotional intelligence for this item. For example,

if a participant selected option 1 ("no disgust") for item 4, and 60% of the whole sample also selected option 1, this participant would obtain a score of .60 for item 4. After that, the scores of all items were averaged in order to get the overall score of MSCEIT.

EmIn (Emotional intelligence) is a self-report measure by D. Lyusin (2006). It consists of 46 questions that describe five domains: understanding of one's own and others' emotions, management of one's own and others' emotions and control of expression. It showed a good reliability (.80), and confirmatory factor analysis revealed a 5-factor model, as proposed by the author. EI (Emotional intelligence) questionnaire by N. Hall (retrieved from Ilyin, 2001) is a wide-spread self-report measure of emotional intelligence. However, to our knowledge, it has not been subject to psychometric analysis. It consists of 30 questions with a 6-score scale.

DNA was extracted from buccal cells and the genotyping procedure was carried out using PCR ("Biological Solutions and Technologies", Russia, Moscow). The analyzed DNA sections were the following: BDNF gene (rs6265, 68690G>A, Val66Met; genotypes: Val/Val, Val/Met, Met/Met), COMT gene (rs4680, 23753G>A, Val158Met; genotypes: Val/Val, Val/Met, Met/Met), HTR2A gene (rs6311 (Tr2), 4692G>A, genotypes: G/G, G/A, A/A; rs6313 (Tr3), 6230C>T, genotypes: C/C, C/T, T/T), DRD2 gene (rs1800497, 32806C>T; genotypes: : C/C, C/T, T/T).

As the data were distributed normally (all ps > .05, according to Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests), parametric analyses were applied. Correlations between emotional intelligence measures were calculated by the Pearson coefficient. Parametric analysis of variance with the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used to study the effects of different genotypes on the emotional intelligence scores. Partial eta-squared (np2) was computed as a measure of effect size.

Results

Emotional intelligence measures

We found a positive correlation between the two self-report measures of emotional intelligence, EmIn by Lyusin and EI by Hall, r = .44, p < .001. Correlations between these self-report measures and MSCEIT were not significant (p > .05) (Table 1), despite research previously demonstrating a small correlation between MSCEIT and some self-report measures of emotional intelligence (Zeidner et al., 2005; Farrelly et al., 2007).

Table 1

Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients between emotional intelligence measures

Emotional intelligence measures Mean SD EmIn EI

MSCEIT .393 .044 .09 .08

EmIn by Lyusin 79.8 12.5 .44*

EI by Hall 25.1 27.4

Note. *p<.05

Association between СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2, HTR2A genotypes and emotional intelligence

We found that COMT genotypes had an effect on MSCEIT (F = 3.20, p = .042, np2 = 0.02 (Figure 1). Post-hoc analysis showed that the carriers of the Val/Met genotype (M = .406, 95% CI: .397 - .415) had a higher level of emotional intelligence than carriers of the Val/Val genotype (M = .391, 95% CI: .381 - .401). The carriers of the Met/Met genotype did not statistically differ from other subsamples to any significant extent, although their emotional intelligence was the lowest (M = .387, 95% CI: .368 - .407). The study did not reveal BDNF genotypes to have an effect on MSCEIT scores (p > .05) (Figure 2).

.46

K

uu .44

C

to

.42

<u

u

c

OJ

.40

-¡Ü

c .38

15

c

o .35

o

£

LU .34

.32

Val/Met

Val/Val COMT

Met/Met

Figure 1. Emotional intelligence (MSCEIT) as according to the COMT genotypes Val/Met, Val/Val and Met/Met. Note. Points reflect means; whiskers reflect SDs. Horizontal lines mean that there are significant differences between indicated values.

.!? -40 ■U

.£ .38 "(5

J .35

<3

£

m .34

Val/Met

Val/Val BDNF

Met/Met

Figure 2. Emotional intelligence (MSCEIT) as according to the BDNF genotypes Val/Val, Val/Met and Met/Met. Note. Points reflect means; whiskers reflect SDs. Horizontal lines mean that there are significant differences between indicated values.

We found that differences in HTR2A genotypes did influence the MSCEIT score, F = 3.24, p = .041, np2 = 0.03 (Figure 3). Post-hoc analysis showed that carriers of A/A (M = .414; 95% CI: .398 - .430) had a higher level of emotional intelligence than carriers of G/G (M = .391; 95% CI: .382 - .400). The A/G carriers did not differ from other subsamples in emotional intelligence (M = .401, 95% CI: .391 - .411).

We also noted that different DRD2 genotypes affected the MSCEIT score, F = 3.42, p = .034, np2 = 0.03 (Figure 4). Post-hoc analysis showed that C/C carriers (M = .405, 95% CI: .395 - .414) had a higher emotional intelligence than C/T carriers (M = .387; 95% CI: .377 - .397). Carriers of the T/T genotype did not differ from other subsamples in emotional intelligence (M = .401; 95% CI: .386 - .415).

As for the self-report measures of emotional intelligence, we did not find that differences in COMT, BDNF, HTR2A, DRD2 genotypes had any effect on EmIn or EI score (all ps > .05). Table 2 represents means and SDs of EmIn or EI scores in carriers of different COMT, BDNF, HTR2A, and DRD2 genotypes.

.32 -

A/G G/G A/A

HTR2A

Figure 3. Emotional intelligence (MSCEIT) as according to the HTR2A genotypes A/G, G/G and A/A.

Note. Points reflect means; whiskers reflect SDs. Horizontal lines mean that there are significant differences between indicated values.

-*-

.44 ш 42

U 42

S .40

"ф .38 "(5

С .35 О

£ .34 .32

C/C C/T T/T

DRD2

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Figure 4. Emotional intelligence (MSCEIT) as according to the DRD2 genotypes C/C, C/T and T/T.

Note. Points reflect means; whiskers reflect SDs. Horizontal lines mean that there are significant differences between indicated values.

Table 2

EmIn or EI scores in carriers of different COMT, BDNF, HTR2A, and DRD2 genotypes.

EmIn by Lusin EI by Hall

mean SD mean SD

Val/Val 80.7 14.4 26.3 28.4

COMT Val/Met 79.1 12.8 26.6 26.1

Met/Met 80 10.5 27.9 31.6

Val/Val 79.2 12.8 24.8 26.8

BDNF Val/Met 81.2 16 26.8 28.1

Met/Met 87.1 15.6 41.2 26.9

A/A 80.2 9.9 30.9 22.9

HTR2A A/G 80.2 14.9 26.9 28.3

G/G 79.3 12.7 25.1 28

C/C 80 13 24.8 27.8

DRD2 C/T 79.9 13.8 28.9 27.8

T/T 80.3 13.1 27.3 26.8

Discussion

In this paper we conducted a comparative study of emotional intelligence in carriers of various genotypes of the COMT, BDNF, HTR2A, and DRD2 genes. The objective method of measuring emotional intelligence (the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso test) showed much greater validity than self-reported (subjective) questionnaires, with regards to genetic differences.

The catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene. We found that the Val/Met genotype (which determines the average duration of monoamine residence in the synaptic space) was associated with a higher level of emotional intelligence. In general, the heterozygous genotype of the COMT gene is associated with a higher adaptive potential and participants' self-reports of effective emotion regulation in everyday life (Weiss et al., 2016; Bunyaeva et al., 2016), which is consistent with our data which saw a significantly higher level of emotional intelligence in representatives of this group. The catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene influences changes in amygdala activity and amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during face processing (Lonsdorf et al., 2011; Surguladze, S. A. et al., 2012). It has been shown that insufficient or excessive catecholaminergic activity associated with the COMT genotype is undesirable for the ability to maintain stable focus of attention during visual perception (Shalev et al., 2019), which can be important for visual recognition of emotions.

The brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. It is known that the presence of the Met allele of the BDNF gene is associated with reduced brain-derived neurotrophic factor secretion, which is associated with the transition from plasticity to stability in neural networks (Egan et al., 2003; Brown et al., 2020). This allows us to assume the possible existence of links between the level of emotional intelligence and the presence or absence of the Met allele. However, in our work we found that the genotypes of the BDNF gene are not associated with the level of emotional intelligence.

The dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene. In this work, it was found that the presence of the major C/C genotype (with a high number of dopamine D2 receptors on the presynaptic membrane) is associated with higher levels of emotional intelligence according to the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso test. At the same time, the heterozygous C/T genotype (with an average number of dopamine D2 receptors on the presynaptic membrane) is associated with a lower level of emotional intelligence. Consequently, a high number of dopamine D2 receptors on the presynaptic membrane is associated with higher levels of emotional intelligence. The dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene is a part of the dopamine neurotransmitter system. The presence of at least one minor allele in the genotype leads to a decrease in the number of dopamine D2 receptors on the presynaptic membrane and increases a person's propensity to develop various addictions (Kibitov, 2011). It was shown that the presence of minor DRD2 alleles in both healthy subjects and patients with schizophrenia was associated with worse emotion recognition (Alfimova et al., 2019).

The 5-hydroxytryptamine receptor 2A (HTR2A) gene. We found that the presence of a minor A/A genotype was associated with a high level of emotional intelligence. The major G/G genotype was associated with a lower level of emotional in-

telligence. Our results do not contradict the results of another study, where it was indicated that the G allele of rs6311 was related to higher scores on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale compared to the AA genotype (Li et al., 2020). In studies on samples of patients with schizophrenia, it was shown that schizophrenia patients more often carry the major genotype C/C of the HTR2A gene to a statistically significant extent (this corresponds to G/G, since the loci rs6313 C>T and rs6311 G>A are in non-equilibrium coupling), while having a low density of serotonin 2A receptors in the brain (Polesskaya et al., 2002; Parsons et al., 2004). In a recent study performed on healthy subjects using positron emission tomography, there were no statistically significant differences in the density of serotonin 2A receptors between carriers of different genotypes of the HTR2A gene, but the trend (without statistical significance) was the same as in the samples of patients (Spies et al., 2020). In the same vein, we can assume that the association of the minor A/A genotype and a high level of emotional intelligence may be accompanied by a tendency to a higher number of serotonin 2A receptors on the presynaptic membrane and may be affected by epigen-etic mechanisms. For example, studies of the epigenetic mechanisms of depression, such as DNA methylation, as well as genotype-environment interaction, have shown that depression is associated with the exposure to various environmental risk factors, such as stress, childhood abuse, and stressful life events (Lin, E. et al., 2019). In a study by Brown and colleagues, devoted to the analysis of the epigenetics of emotion regulation disorders, it was shown that the most important role in these processes is that of methylation, which regulates the activity of genes without changing the DNA sequence. The important role of genes in the implementation of the serotonin, dopamine and noradrenaline systems, as well as the limbic-hypothalamic axis of the adrenal glands (l-HPA) is also noted (Brown et al., 2020).

Conclusion

On the basis of the conducted research, some generalizations can be formulated. This work was one of the first studies to investigate the influence of genetic factors on emotional intelligence in healthy respondents. The carriers of the Val/Met genotype of the COMT gene, A/A genotype of the HTR2A gene and C/C genotype of the DRD2 gene showed the highest levels of emotional intelligence. These differences, according to our data, may also be caused by genetic differences in serotonin and dopamine neurotransmitter systems, which control the duration of both serotonin and dopamine in the intersynaptic space. These include the 5-hydroxytryptamine 2A receptor (HTR2A) gene, the dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene, and the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) gene.

The results obtained can form the basis for further research, possibly involving more participants. Recently, a neural model of emotional intelligence was proposed, which offered plausible targets for improving emotional intelligence with training (Smith et al., 2018). Taking into account genetic characteristics will make such training more individualized.

Emotional intelligence research is currently one of the most dynamically developing areas of modern psychological science, and this trend only intensifies with the

increase in the number of risks faced by a modern person (e.g., restrictions due to a pandemic). The level of emotional intelligence in an individual can be regarded as the single most important personal psychological resource that can ensure successful adaptation in society, since it allows its owner to recognize and express, regulate, and manage both his own emotional manifestations and the emotions of other people. In this work, based on a sample of young people in the South of Russia living in the Rostov region and the republics of Kabardino-Balkaria and Karachay-Cherkessia, a comparative study of the diagnosis of emotional intelligence using three different methods was carried out. These methods were the self-report questionnaires of Hall and Lyusin and the objective methodology of Mayer-Salovey-Caruso.

The manifestations of emotional intelligence can have their own characteristics, caused by both genetic factors (depending on the frequency of occurrence of ho-mozygous dominant and recessive or heterozygous genotypes) and environmental influences (training and education in ways of manifesting emotions). The study of emotional intelligence acquires particular relevance if and when it is based on the use of natural scientific methods that allow a deeper understanding of the nature of the studied psychological phenomena.

Limitations

Finally, we admit that our work has several limitations. Thus, we have not equalized the gender and ages of our subsamples. Future studies could collect data for more genes, such as the oxytocin receptor (OXTR), which is associated with empathy traits (Luo et al., 2015), or the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) related to social behavior (Uzefovsky et al., 2014) and others.. We also suggest that larger samples could be examined in order to expand upon the knowledge of the association between genetics and personality.

Ethics Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants followed the ethical standards of the Southern Federal University research committee and the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments, or comparable ethical standards. All participants were informed about the purpose of the study, and gave their voluntary consent to participate in it.

Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, supervision, project administration, writing and manuscript revision were carried out by Elena V. Vorobyeva. Methodology, review, investigation, data analyses and manuscript revision were carried out by Ekaterina M. Kovsh and Vladimir V. Kosonogov. Authors have read and endorsed the published version of the manuscript.

92 Vorobyeva, E.V., Kovsh, E.M., Kosonogov, V.V. Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Russian Foundation of Basic Research (Project No. 18-013-01019).

References

Alekseeva, D., Nikolaev, S., Babenko, V., & Yavna, D. (2019). Contribution of spatial modulations of brightness gradients to face perception. XVI European Congress of Psychology, 1805-1805. https:// www. elibrary. ru/item.asp?id=41293513 Alfimova, M.V., Golimbet, V.E., Korovaitseva, G.I., Lezheiko, T.V., Tikhonov, D.V., Ganisheva, T.K., Berezin, N.B., Snegireva, A.A., & Shemyakina, T.K. (2019). The Role of the Interaction between the NMDA and Dopamine Receptor Genes in Impaired Recognition of Emotional Expression in Schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 49, 153-158. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s11055-018-0709-y

Andriushchenko, E.A., Golshtein, I.R., & Sherbakova, O.V. (2020). The fullness of understanding of ambiguous texts in people with different levels of emotional intelligence and theory of mind. Voprosy Psikhologii, 6, 69-80.

Anglim, J., Morse, G., Dunlop, P.D., Minbashian, A., Marty, A. (2020). Predicting trait emotional intelligence from HEXACO personality: Domains, facets, and the general factor of personality. Journal Pers., 88(2), 324-338. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12493 Barabanschikov, V.A., & Zherdev, I.Y. (2019). Visual Perception of Facial Emotional Expressions during

Saccades. Behavioral Sciences, 9, 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120131 Barbey, A.K., Colom, R., & Grafman, J. (2014). Distributed neural system for emotional intelligence revealed by lesion mapping. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 265-272. https://doi. org/10.1093/scan/nss124

Brown, D.T., Vickers, J.C., Stuart, K.E., Cechova, K., & Ward, D.D. (2020). The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Modulates Resilience of Neurological Functioning to Brain Ageing and Dementia: A Narrative Review. Brain Sciences, 10(4), Article Number 195. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10040195 Brown, M., Conradt, E., & Crowell, S.E. (2020). Epigenetic Foundations of Emotion Dysregula-tion. The Oxford Handbook of Emotion Dysregulation, 221. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxford-hb/9780190689285.013.16 Bunyaeva, M.V., Kovsh, E.M., Skirtach, I.A., & Il'in, A.B. (2016). Otrazhenie sochetaniya genotipov po polimorfnym lokusam COMT Val158Met i MAOA -uVNTR v psihologicheskih osoben-nostyah devushek [The reflection of the combination of genotypes of polymorphic loci of COMT Val158Met and MAOA-UVNTR in psychological features of girls]. Rossiyskiy Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal [Russian Psychological Journal], 13, 218-231. https://doi.org/10.21702/rpj.2016.4.13 Caldu, X., Ottino-Gonzalez, J., Sanchez-Garre, C., Hernan, I., Tor, E., Sender-Palacios, M.J., Dreher, J.C., Garolera, M., & Jurado, M.A. (2019). Effect of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val (158)Met polymorphism on theory of mind in obesity. European Eating Disorders Review, 27(4), 401-409. DOI 10.1002/erv.2665

Cha, C.B., & Nock, M.K. (2009). Emotional intelligence is a protective factor for suicidal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 422-430. https://doi. org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181984f44 Dewi, Z.L., Halim, M.S., & Derksen, J. (2018). Emotional intelligence competences of three different ethnic groups in Indonesia. Asian Ethnicity, 19, 36-58. https://doi.org/10.1080/14631369.2017.! 310615

Egan, M.F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J.H., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Bertolino, A., & Lu, B. (2003). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell, 112, 257-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0092-8674(03)00035-7 Ermakov, P.N., Kovsh, E.M., Vorobeva, E.V., & Kosonogov, V.V. (2019). Association of different BDNF, COMT, HTR2A and DRD2 genotypes with levels of emotional intelligence and brain activity characteristics in Armenians. Behavior Genetics, 49, 518-518.https://apps.webofknowledge.com/ full_record.do?product=WOS&search_mode=GeneralSearch&qid=2&SID=D5GU2dU2a8okwu Dmxow&page=1&doc=1

Farrelly, D., & Austin, E.J. (2007). Ability EI as an intelligence? Associations of the MSCEIT with performance on emotion processing and social tasks and with cognitive ability. Cognition and Emotion, 21(5), 1043-1063. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601069404 Ferguson, FJ., & Austin, E.J. (2010). Associations of trait and ability emotional intelligence with performance on Theory of Mind tasks in an adult sample. Personality and Idividual Differences, 49(5), 414-418. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.paid.2010.04.009 Fernández, M.M., Brito, C.J., Miarka, B., & Díaz-de-Durana, A.L. (2020). Anxiety and Emotional Intelligence: Comparisons Between Combat Sports, Gender and Levels Using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale and the Inventory of Situations and Anxiety Response. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article Number 130. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00130 Gadow, K.D., Pinsonneault, J.K., Perlman, G., & Sadee, W. (2014). Association of dopamine gene variants, emotion dysregulation and ADHD in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(7), 1658-1665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.007 Gohier, B., Senior, C., Radua, J., El-Hage, W., Reichenberg, A., Proitsi, P., & Surguladze, S.A. (2014). Genetic modulation of the response bias towards facial displays of anger and happiness. European Psychiatry, 29, 197-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.03.003 Halicka-Maslowska, J., Szewczuk-Boguslawska, M., Pawlak-Adamska, E., Adamska, A., & Misiak, B. (2021). Effects of variation in dopaminergic genes on the level of aggression and emotional intelligence in adolescents with conduct disorder. Archives of psychiatry and psychotherapy, 23 (2), 15-23. https://doi.org/10.12740/APP/128451 Hodzic, S., Scharfen, J., Ripoll, P., Holling, H., & Zenasni, F. (2018). How efficient are emotional intelligence trainings: A meta-analysis. Emotion Review, 10, 138-148. https://doi. org/10.1177/1754073917708613 Hosseini, M.A., Khankeh, H.R., & Hosseinzadeh, S. (2012). The effect of emotional intelligence education on job conflict in nurses. Journal of Health Promotion Management, 1, 7-16. http://nmj. umsha.ac.ir/article-1-1855-en.html Ilyin, E.P. (2001). Emotsii i chuvstva [Emotions and feelings]. Saint Petersburg: Piter. Israelashvili, J., Oosterwijk, S., Sauter, D., & Fischer, A. (2019). Knowing me, knowing you: emotion differentiation in oneself is associated with recognition of others' emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 33(7), 1461-1471. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1577221 Katrushova, L., Yalanska, S., Rudenko, L., & Katrushov, O. (2019). Peculiarities of the process of psychological adaptation of foreign students of Ukrainian higher education institutions of medical profile, role of emotional intelligence in the socialization process. Wiadomosci Lekarskie, 72, 1930-1934. https://doi.org/10.36740/WLek201910114 Kibitov, A.O. (2011). Genetika narkologicheskih zabolevanij: molekulyarno-geneticheskij profil' dofam-inovoj nejromediatornoj sistemy pri alkogolizme i opijnoj narkomanii [The genetics of addiction diseases: molecular genetic profile of the dopamine neurotransmitter system in alcoholism and opioid addiction]. Narkologiya [Narcology], 10, 25-42. https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=27719974 Kim, S.J., Kang, J.I., Namkoong, K., & Song, D.H. (2011). The effects of serotonin transporter promoter and monoamine oxidase A gene polymorphisms on trait emotional intelligence. Neuropsychobio-logy, 64, 224--230. https://doi.org/10.1159/000327705 Kosonogov, V.V., Vorobyeva, E., Kovsh, E., & Ermakov, P.N. (2019). A review of neurophysiological and genetic correlates of emotional intelligence. International Journal of Cognitive Research in Science, Engineering and Education, 7, 137-142. https://doi.org/10.5937/ijcrsee1901137K

Kundi, Y.M., & Badar, K. (2021). Interpersonal conflict and counterproductive work behavior: the moderating roles of emotional intelligence and gender. International Journal of Conflict Management, 32(3), 514-534. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJCMA-10-2020-0179 Li, X.H., He, L.L., Liu, J.T., Guo, W.X., Wang, Q.H., Fang, P.P., Yang, X., Zhang, M.F., Wang, C.L., & Gong, P.Y. (2020). The rs6311 of serotonin receptor 2A (5-HT2A) gene is associated with alexi-thymia and mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders, 272, 277-282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jad.2020.03.153

Lin, E., & Tsai, S.J. (2019). Epigenetics and depression: an update. Psychiatry Investigation, 16, 654-661.

https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2019.07.17.2 Lischke, A., Pahnke, R., Konig, J., Homuth, G., Hamm, A.O., & Wendt, J. (2019). COMTVal158Met Genotype Affects Complex Emotion Recognition in Healthy Men and Women. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, Article Number 1007. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.01007 Liu, W., Peeters, N., Fernández, G., & Kohn, N. (2020). Common neural and transcriptional correlates of inhibitory control underlie emotion regulation and memory control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(5), 523-536. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa073 Liu, X.Y., Hildebrandt, A., Recio, G., Sommer, W., Cai, X.X., & Wilhelm, O. (2017). Individual Differences in the Speed of Facial Emotion Recognition Show Little Specificity but Are Strongly Related with General Mental Speed: Psychometric, Neural and Genetic Evidence. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, Article Number 149. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00149 Lonsdorf, T.B., Golkar, A., Lindstom, K.M., Fransson, P., Schalling, M., & Ohman, A., et al. (2011). 5-HTTLPR and COMTval158met genotype gate amygdala reactivity and habituation. Biol. Psychol. 87, 106-112. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.biopsycho.2011.02.014 Luo, S., Li, B., Ma, Y., Zhang, W., Rao, Y., & Han, S. (2015). Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy-related brain activity. Neurolmage, 110, 22-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroim-age.2015.01.042

Lyusin, D.V. (2006). Novaya metodika dlya izmereniya emocional'nogo intellekta: oprosnik EmIn [A new method of emotional intelligence measurement: EmIn questionnaire]. Psihologicheskaya di-agnostika [Psychological diagnostics], 4, 3-22. https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=26114976 Lyusin, D., & Ovsyannikova, V. (2016). Measuring two aspects of emotion recognition ability: Accuracy vs. sensitivity. Learning and Individual Differences, 52, 129-136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lin-dif.2015.04.010

Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D.R. (2004). Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Findings, and Implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15(3), 197-215. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02 Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D.R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence with the

MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion, 3, 97-105. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97. Pankratova, A.A. (2019). Ability models of emotional intelligence and survey of diagnostic methods.

Voprosy Psikhologii, 6, 151-162. Pankratova A.A., & Lyusin D.V. (2016). Pravila proyavleniya emocij v russkoj i azerbajdzhanskoj kul'turah [Emotional display rules in Russian and Azerbaijani cultures]. Psikhologicheskie Issledo-vaniya [Psychological Studies], 9(48), 11. http://psystudy.ru Parsons, M.J., D'Souza, U.M., Arranz, M.J., Kerwin, R.W., & Makoff, A.J. (2004). The-1438A/G polymorphism in the 5-hydroxytryptamine type 2A receptor gene affects promoter activity. Biological Psychiatry, 56(6), 406-410. https://doi.org/10.1016Aj.biopsych.2004.06.020 Pavlova, E.M., & Kornilova, T.V. (2013). Creativity and tolerance for uncertainty predict the engagement of emotional intelligence in personal decision making. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 6, 34-46. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2013.0403 Pecina, M., Mickey, B. J., Love, T., Wang, H., Langenecker, S.A., Hodgkinson, C., Shen, P. H., Villafuerte, S., Hsu, D., & Weisenbach, S.L. (2013). DRD2 polymorphisms modulate reward and emotion processing, dopamine neurotransmission and openness to experience. Cortex, 49(3), 877-890. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.01.010 Polesskaya, O.O., & Sokolov, B.P. (2002). Differential expression of the "C" and "T" alleles of the 5-HT2A receptor gene in the temporal cortex of normal individuals and schizophrenics. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 67(6), 812-822. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.10173

Qin, Y., Kang, J., Jiao, Z., Wang, Y., Wang, J., Wang, H., Feng, J., Jin, L., Wang, F., & Gong, X. (2020). Polygenic risk for autism spectrum disorder affects left amygdala activity and negative emotion in schizophrenia. Translational Psychiatry, 0(1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-01001-2 Quan, R., Wu, Z., Guo, W., He, L., Fang, P., & Gong, P. (2021). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impacts victim's moral emotions following interpersonal transgression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 62(1), 7-12. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12678 Raz, S., Dan, O., Arad, H., & Zysberg, L. (2013). Behavioral and neural correlates of emotional intelligence: An Event-Related Potentials (ERP) study. Brain Research, 1526, 44-53. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.048 Redlich, R., Schneider, I., Kerkenberg, N., Opel, N., Bauhaus, J., Enneking, V., Repple, J., Leehr, E.J., Grotegerd, D., Kähler, C., Förster, K., & Dohm, K. (2020). The role of BDNF methylation and Val66Met in amygdala reactivity during emotion processing. Human Brain Mapping, 41(3), 594-604. https:// doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24825 Sergienko, E., Khlevnaya, E., & Osipenko, E. (2020). Development of an objective methodology for measurement of emotional intelligence. Emotional intelligence and socio-demographics of employees in Russian organizations. Organizational Psychology, 10(1), 45-61. https://www.elibrary.ru/item. asp?id=42801866

Sergienko, E.A., & Vetrova, I.I. (2010). Test Dzh. Mejera, P. Seloveya, D. Karuzo «Emocional'nyj intellekt» (MSCEIT v. 2.0): Rukovodstvo [The test of J. Mayer, P. Salovey and D. Caruso «Emotional intelligence» (MSCEIT v. 2.0): Manual]. Moscow: Institute of Psychology RAS. Smith, R., Killgore, W.D.S., Alkozei, A., & Lane, R.D. (2018). A neuro-cognitive process model of emotional intelligence. Biological Psychology, 139, 131-151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsy-cho.2018.10.012

Surguladze, S.A., Radua, J., El-Hage, W., Gohier, B., Sato, J. R., Kronhaus, D.M., Proitsi, P., Powell, J., & Phillips, M.L. (2012). Interaction of catechol O-methyltransferase and serotonin transporter genes modulates effective connectivity in a facial emotion-processing circuitry. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e70. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2011.69 Shalev, N., Vangkilde, S., Neville, M.J., Tunbridge, E.M., Nobre, A.C., & Chechlacz, M. (2019). Dissociable Catecholaminergic Modulation of Visual Attention: Differential Effects of Catechol-O-Methyltransferase and Dopamine Beta-Hydroxylase Genes on Visual Attention. Neuroscience, 412, 175-189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.068 6645579 Spies, M., Nasser, A., Ozenne, B., Jensen, P. S., Knudsen, G.M., & Fisher, P.M. (2020). Common HTR2A variants and 5-HTTLPR are not associated with human in vivo serotonin 2A receptor levels. Human Brain Mapping, 41(16), 4518-4528. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25138. Takeuchi, H., Tomita, H., Taki, Y., Kikuchi, Y., Ono, C., Yu, Z., & Miyauchi, C.M. (2015). The associations among the dopamine D2 receptor Taq1, emotional intelligence, creative potential measured by divergent thinking, and motivational state and these associations' sex differences. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 912. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00912 Tian, T., Li, J., Zhang, G.L., Wang, J., Liu, D., Wan, C.H., Fang, J.C., Wu, D., Zhou, Y.R., & Zhu, W.Z. (2021). Effects of childhood trauma experience and BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on brain plasticity relate to emotion regulation. Behavioural Brain Research, 398, Article Number 112949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112949 Understanding emotional intelligence / J.T. Lanthem (Editor). Series: Psychology Research Progress.

2021. BISAC: PSY000000. 199 p. Uzefovsky, F., Shalev, I., Israel, S., Edelman, S., Raz, Y., Perach-Barzilay, N., Mankuta, D., Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Knafo, A., & Ebstein, R.P. (2014). The Dopamine D4 receptor gene shows a gender-sensitive association with cognitive empathy: Evidence from two independent samples. Emotion, 14(4), 712-721. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036555 van der Linden, D., Pekaar, K.A., Bakker, A.B., Schermer, J.A., Vernon, P.A., Dunkel, C.S., & Petrides, K.V. (2017). Overlap between the general factor of personality and emotional intelligence: A metaanalysis. Psychological bulletin, 143(1), 36-52. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000078 Vorobyeva, E.V., Ermakov, P.N., Borokhovski, E.F., Kovsh, E.M., & Stoletniy, A.S. (2020). Association between categorization of emotionally-charged and neutral visual scenes and parameters of event-

related potentials in carriers of COMT, HTR2A, BDNF gene polymorphisms. F1000Research, 9(446), 446. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.22503.2 Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Stroud, C.B., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R.E., Adam, E.K., Redei, E.E., Hammen, C., & Craske, M.G. (2015). Additive genetic risk from five serotonin system polymorphisms interacts with interpersonal stress to predict depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 776-790. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000098 Weiss, E.M., Freudenthaler, H.H., Fink, A., Reiser, E. M., Niederstatter, H., Nagl, S., Parson, W., & Pa-pousek, I. (2014). Differential Influence of 5-HTTLPR — Polymorphism and COMT Val158Met — Polymorphism on Emotion Perception and Regulation in Healthy Women. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(5), 516-524. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771400023X Zeidner, M., Shani-Zinovich, I., Matthews, G., & Roberts, R.D. (2005). Assessing emotional intelligence in gifted and non-gifted high school students: Outcomes depend on the measure. Intelligence, 33(4), 369-391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.03.001

Original manuscript received December 13, 2020 Revised manuscript accepted June 7, 2022 First published online June 30, 2022

To cite this article: Vorobyeva, E.V., Kovsh, E.M., Kosonogov, V.V. (2022). Emotional Intelligence in Carriers of Different СОМТ, BDNF, DRD2 and HTR2A Genotypes. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 15(2), 83-96. DOI: 10.11621/pir.2022.0206

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.