THE ROLE OF MATERNAL EDUCATION IN REGULATING GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILD'S LANGUAGE COMPETENCIES
Chernov DN ^
Department of General Psychology and Pedagogy, Faculty of Psychology and Sociology, Pirogov Russian National Research Medical University, Moscow, Russia
Understanding the role of the environment in the dynamics of gene-environment interactions shaping psychological traits of the child is one of the central issues of contemporary psychogenetics. The socioeconomic status of the parents (education in particular) is a critical factor regulating the share of environmental and genetic influences on the child's cognitive abilities. This work is a study of phenotypic associations between the results of the subtests of the Heidelberg Speech Development Test designed to measure children's speech and language competence, by computing genotypic and environmental correlations between its components. Children were divided into groups based on the educational level of their mothers (medium and high); each group was analyzed separately. For our analysis we used the twin method: the group of twins born to mothers with medium-level education included 17 monozygotic and 11 dizygotic twin pairs; the group of children born to highly educated mothers was comprised of 17 monozygotic and 22 dizygotic twin pairs. All children were aged from 7 years to 8 years and 11 months. Family report forms revealed an association between maternal education and individualized approach to the upbringing of each of the twins. It was shown that in families with highly educated mothers, differences in the upbringing strategies improve the development of language and speech competencies of the child, strengthen the relationship between various language competencies, increase the contribution of the genotype to and decrease the role of the general family environment in this relationship.
Keywords: twin method, genetic correlation, education, psychogenetics, socioeconomic status, environmental correlation, language competence
[>3 Correspondence should be addressed: Dmitry Chernov
ul. Ostrovityanova, d. 1, Moscow, Russia, 117997; [email protected]
Received: 14.04.2017 Accepted: 07.05.2017
РОЛЬ ОБРАЗОВАТЕЛЬНОГО СТАТУСА МАТЕРИ В ИЗМЕНЕНИИ ГЕНОТИП-СРЕДОВЫХ СООТНОШЕНИЙ В СТРУКТУРЕ ЯЗЫКОВЫХ ХАРАКТЕРИСТИК
Д. Н.Чернов и
Кафедра общей психологии и педагогики, психолого-социальный факультет,
Российский национальный исследовательский медицинский университет имени Н. И. Пирогова, Москва
Изучение роли средовых факторов в изменении генотип-средовых соотношений по психологическим характеристикам является актуальной задачей современной психогенетики. Важнейший фактор изменения генотип-средовых соотношений по когнитивным способностям — социоэкономический, и в частности образовательный, статус родителей. Исследовались причины фенотипических взаимосвязей между субтестами Гейдельбергского теста речевого развития ребенка путем подсчета генотипических и средовых корреляций между одноименными характеристиками. Анализ проводился раздельно в подгруппах детей из семей со средним и высоким образовательным статусом их матерей. Применяли близнецовый метод: в подгруппу близнецов из семей со средним образовательным статусом матерей вошли 17 монозиготных и 11 дизиготных пар; подгруппу детей из семей с высоким образовательным статусом матерей составили 17 монозиготных и 22 дизиготных пары. Возраст детей — 7 лет — 8 лет 11 мес. На основании анкетных данных показано, что образовательный статус связан с субъектной активностью матерей в вопросах индивидуализации воспитания близнецов в паре. В семьях с высоким образовательным статусом матерей эта установка приводит к возрастанию уровня языкового развития детей, росту структурной связанности различных языковых характеристик, увеличению удельного веса общего генотипического фактора и снижению роли общесемейной среды в объяснении природы этой структурной связанности.
Ключевые слова: близнецовый метод, генетическая корреляция, образовательный статус, психогенетика, социоэкономический статус, средовая корреляция, языковая компетенция
Для корреспонденции: Чернов Дмитрий Николаевич
ул. Островитянова, д. 1, г. Москва, 117997; [email protected]
Статья получена: 14.04.2017 Статья принята к печати: 07.05.2017
Personal shapes of various psychological characteristics in children develop under a significant influence of the socioeconomic status (SES) of their parents. The components of SES are family income, educational and professional status of parents. Favorable conditions for the development of children in families with high SES mean a number of things: children can receive high-quality food and medical care; families can choose housing that is more environmentally sound; children enjoy a cognition stimulating environment; the parent-child relationship is harmonic and the upbringing attitudes are positive [1]. In our country, using SES as a criterion for group formation in research studies is hindered by somewhat incomplete questionnaire data obtained from the parent, who prefer to give just a general description of their families' financial standing. Grouping research participants by educational status (ES) of parents turns out to be a productive way of studying the contribution of SES to individual psychological traits [2].
Parents' SES is a factor in shaping individual peculiarities of language characteristics. The relationship between parent's SES and child's language skills development manifests at the age of 1.5 years. By the age of 3, mother's SES and education are positively related to the size and diversity of child's active vocabulary, ability to understand the language, average length of utterance in morphemes, variety of word combinations, compound and complex structures used [3-7]. Children of preschool and school age coming from families with high SES have a large vocabulary, use grammatically and syntactically more complex sentences in their speech, progress significantly in developing reading skills, better cope with verbal tasks than their peers from families with low SES [5, 8-11].
Parents with high SES and ES create an environment that fosters children's language skills development. Mothers from such families are verbally responsive, communicate more with their children and encourage them to communicate, keep up topical conversations longer, tend to avoid giving directive instructions and react to the statements made by children more lively. The lexical and grammatical composition of their speech is rich and contains more information about the surrounding objects. The amount of time high-SES mothers allocate to child-parent interaction is as important for child's language skills development as the average characteristics of mother's speech. Intergroup differences in the volume of active vocabulary possessed by infants from families with different SES are almost completely dependent on the quality of verbal environment [5, 12-14]. At the same time, with children aged 1 through 4 the role of SES in the development of individual differences regarding the richness of active vocabulary diminishes when lexical and syntactic complexity of mother's speech is taken into account [6]. Creating a more development-oriented environment in low-SES families produces a beneficial effect on the development of language mastery [15, 16]. Parental education and income affect parent-child interactions and make a relatively independent contribution to the verbal development of a child between 1.5 and 3 years of age [17, 18]. The studies suggest that SES of parents is a correlate of the level of parental activity aimed at improving the environment that promotes development of language skills.
These studies provide an understanding of the role of a family's SES as an environmental factor affecting variability of language skills. However, language competencies can also be affected by genetic factors. Psychogenetics, a cross between psychology and genetics, aims to research the roles played by hereditary and environmental factors (and their interaction) in the formation of individual variations of psychological characteristics. Studies of the nature of interindividual variability
of language competencies prove that this variability is influenced by environmental and genetic factors [20].
An important share of psychogenetic research efforts aims to study the role of environmental factors in the changes occurring in genotype-environment interactions. The researchers rely on the bioecological model proposed by Bronfenbrenner and Ceci, which assumes that the wealth of development resources provided by the child's immediate social environment has a lasting effect on the contribution of distal environmental resources (education, culture, economy) to the development of the child and, furthermore, can influence the expression of developmental genetic predisposition [21]. Scarr believes that environmental characteristics can regulate the ratio of contributions made by genetic and environmental factors to the formation of interindividual variability of psychological characteristics [22].
These assumptions imply that during early ontogeny, individual patterns, like those of cognitive characteristics, must be significantly influenced by factors shared by all family members. With age, the contribution of hereditary factors to the variability of cognitive abilities increases, while the contribution of family-related factors decreases. It was found that SES mediates gene-environment interactions in what concerns children's intellectual development characteristics. Thus, differences in general intelligence observed in 2-year old children from low-SES families can be explained by the influence of family environment. The role of hereditary factors in shaping personal traits increases in children from high-SES families, generally standing on a higher intellectual development level [23, 24]. In 7-year-olds from high-SES families, more than half of phenotypic dispersion of the general intelligence comes from hereditary factors. As for children from low-SES families, about 60 % of their individual differences result from the influence exerted by the general family environment [25]. Psychogenetic studies of verbal intelligence yielded similar results, but they dealt with school-age children [26]. As far as verbal intelligence goes, in early and pre-school age family's SES has a faint influence on the balance of the genetic and environmental contributions [27].
Although most studies confirm the mediating effect of SES on the dynamics of gene-environment interactions in what concerns individual peculiarities of cognitive characteristics, a number of studies report no such effect. Tucker-Drob and Bates provide an explanation for the contradictions. They conducted the meta-analysis of 14 studies revolving around the role of SES in mediating genetic and environmental contributions to the interpersonal variability of intelligence and academic progress and found that such mediation applied to American children. Studies conducted in Western Europe and Australia reveal nothing of the kind. The researchers point out that zero or even negative mediation effect is the product of social policies pursued in these countries, where all population strata have a more or less equal access to quality education and health care [28].
As for the role played by the genetic factors and the environment in the development of language mastery, it was found that the family's SES slightly influences the ratio of their contributions while the age of children is pre-school. Along with home environment orderliness, SES determines only 3-5 % of individual traits while some other factors of the shared environment determine 52-58 % of children's verbal abilities [29, 30]. Research of etiology of individual differences in understanding a written text while reading (8-year old children) revealed that these differences were determined by genetic factors to a large extent and, moreover, contribution of these
genetic factors increased in parallel with school SES (derivative of the SES of pupils' families) where the research participants studied. However, only 7.5 % of individual differences in understanding a written text have anything to do with the correlation between genetic and environmental factors and SES of a school [31]. Parental ES influences the balance between genetic and environmental contributions affecting children of primary school age and adolescents. Comparison of the geneenvironment ratios obtained from the samples of Russian schoolchildren coming from families with high and medium maternal ES shows that individual differences in the degree of Russian language mastery can be explained by the impact of various factors: as a rule, in the first sample (high maternal ES) the contribution of hereditary factors to the interindividual variability of language skills is much higher than in the second sample (medium maternal ES). In turn, in the second sample individual differences in language skills are largely determined by the environment in general [32]. The adolescent sample shows that language understanding is largely influenced by genetic factors in children from families where parents' ES is high. Children coming from families with low ES prove to have their interindividual differences affected by hereditary and general family factors to an equally small degree [33]. Two other studies investigated reading and language skills possessed by adults. Those studies revealed that the higher SES and ES of the families the subjects grew up in, the better are the skills and the more significant is the role of hereditary factors in the formation of the related individual traits. At that, the contribution of the shared environment decreases or remains unchanged [34, 35].
So far, psychogenetic studies offer little information on the role of SES in changing the etiology of the structure of correlations in the set of linguistic characteristics. Previously, we found that close associations between different linguistic characteristics observed in younger schoolchildren should be attributed more to the genetic factors and less to the family environment [36].
The aim of this pilot study was to analyze the interplay between language competencies by studying phenotypic correlations and to assess the dynamics of genetic and environmental contributions to this interplay considering maternal ES. The aim was achieved through counting genotypic and environmental correlations between the characteristics of language development. This pilot study employed the twin method.
METHODS
The Heidelberg speech development test (Ht) was used to assess language skills [37]. Table 1 contains details on test sections, subtests and skills Ht was designed to research. The "raw" subtest scores were translated into standard scores in accordance with the test guide; the age groups were 7 years — 7 years 11 months, 8 years — 8 years 11 months, which allowed eliminating the age difference factor.
Questionnaires filled by mothers allowed assessing upbringing conditions of twins. The questionnaire designed for the study included 11 questions in the "Family Information" section and 52 questions in the "Twins Information" section. The first section offered questions related to age, parents' ES, number of children in the family, household income, professional status of parents, presence of other adults in the family, family leisure activities. Moving to the second section, mothers had to answer questions about the twins, such as related to the first months of their life (birth weight, injuries, long-term illnesses in childhood), early motor and speech development, relationships with peers, adults, parents. A number of questions pertained to the relationship between twins and individualized educational and upbringing strategies parents may have exercised on each twin. With some typical situations from family life described in the question, mothers were asked to choose one of the suggested answers or write down their own answers when the question was open or when choosing an answer from the presented options was difficult. Answers to each question of the survey were presented as dichotomous scales (except when the data could be presented as ordinal scales).
Statistical analysis was conducted using the SPSS 20.0 package. Data on intrapair correlations in twins and heritability coefficients were taken from our previous study and used for calculation of genetic and environmental correlations [32]. ANOVA was applied to assess differences in the levels of studied characteristics. The association between dichotomous characteristics was measured using the 9 coefficient. To obtain phenotypic correlations (r) between the Ht subtests, Pearson's interclass correlation coefficients were calculated for two subgroups of study participants, one gathered from the sample of monozygotic (MZ) twins and the other from the sample of dizygotic (DZ) twins. Each subgroup included one randomly selected twin from each MZ or DZ pair. This was possible because almost all the Ht subtests and final scores were practically the same for MZ and DZ twins (Table 2). The
Table 1. Sections, subtests of the Heidelberg test and corresponding skills
Test section Subtests Skills
Sentence structure Understanding grammatical structures (GS) Sentence
Memorization and repetition of grammatical structures (SR)
Morphological structures Formation of plural nouns (PN) Morpheme
Word formation using the same root (SRW)
Formation of degrees of comparison of adjectives (DCA)
Meaning of sentences Correction of semantically incorrect sentences (CIS) Phrase
Constructing sentences (CS)
Meaning of words Completing analogies (CA) Word
Grouping concepts based on shared features (GC)
Interactive meaning Use of different address forms (addressing the same person differently depending on the context of interpersonal communication) (AF) Utterance / speech act
Establishing the relationship between verbal and non-verbal emotionally loaded information (VNI)
Coding/decoding of intent (CI)
Generalization stage Text (story) memorization and retelling (story) (TM) Text
samples had similar average Ht scores. Correlations were averaged using Z-transforms. Genetic correlations (rg) between the subtests were calculated according to the formulas provided below, separately for MZ and DZ samples; the results were averaged. The formula for genetic correlations:
1/2 (r + r )
_ " Rj Rji'
gi i-
^ rRiir Rjj
r , rRjj — correlation coefficients between i (trait of one member of the twin pair) and j (trait of the other member of the twin pair); rRP rRj — correlation coefficients for the same traits of twin pair members. Average genetic correlation was calculated according to the formula:
r /S2 + r / S2
g(Mzf^ r g(MZ) T g(DZ)j r g(DZ)
'g(MZ)
g(DZ)
rg — genetic correlation coefficient; S r — error of genetic correlation coefficient [38]. Environmental correlations (r ') were calculated based in phenotypic and genotypic correlations and heritability coefficients of Ht subtests, using the following formula:
rg - WhH
V (1-h)(1-H)
rg — genetic correlation coefficient; r — phenotypic correlation coefficient; h and H — correlated traits heritability coefficients [39]. Contributions of family and individual environments to the associations between the subtests were analyzed by comparison of intra-individual and intra-pair cross-correlations of Ht subtests (MZ sample) [40].
The total sample included 68 same-sex pairs of twins aged 7 to 8 years 11 months (x = 8.00, S =0.65), 35 of them monozygotic, 33 — dizygotic pairs, all studying in several public schools of Moscow. 36 pairs of tins were girls, 32 pairs — boys. The researchers contacted school authorities asking if they had twins that fit the study criteria, then contacted parents of such twins and obtained their permission to test children's language skills and gather information about mothers using questionnaires (filled in at family residences). As of the time of the study, all children were classified as putatively healthy: mothers reported no abnormalities in their physical and mental development. Each twin underwent testing separately, during his or her free time. The children were grouped based on maternal ES using questionnaires filled by mothers. In one family, only grandmother was raising the twins, so the pair was excluded from the analysis. 17 MZ and 11 DZ pairs constituted the medium ES subgroup, i. e. their mothers had incomplete secondary, secondary or vocational education. 17 MZ and 22 DZ pairs constituted the high ES subgroup, i.e their mothers had incomplete or complete higher education.
RESULTS
ANOVA reveals that the results of many Ht subtests are different for the medium ES and high ES split-twin groups. The differences are statistically significant, reproducible and independent of the zygosity status (Table 2). Compared to the twins from the medium ES group, the children from the high ES group scored better on the following subtests: Imitation of grammatical structures (p < 0.05 in one group), Word formation (p < 0.001), Formation of degrees of comparison of adjectives
(p < 0.05 in one group), Correction of semantically incorrect sentences (p < 0.05), Sentence construction (p < 0.05), Address forms (p < 0.05), Story memorization (p < 0.005). The differences affect final scores (p < 0.005).
Comparing maternal survey results (medium ES and high ES), we uncovered possible variations in individualized attitudes toward each twin in a pair. Mothers with high ES tended to dress the twins differently (9 = 0.34, p < 0.01), they encouraged the twins to do individual chores more often (9 = 0.33; p < 0.01) and tended to engage one child in doing housework rather than both (9 = 0.30, p < 0.05). Mothers from this group often pointed out that twins were more likely to help around the house independently of each other and not together (9 = 0.25, p < 0.05).
We also calculated phenotypic correlations between Ht subtests in the two samples. Each sample included one of the twins from MZ and DZ pairs whose mothers had high ES (n1 = 39, n2 = 39). Significant associations between the majority of subtests were discovered, related to grammar, morphology, meanings of sentences and work with a text as a whole. Presumably, these subtests constitute a relatively unified factor that we called "Language competence." Also reproducible, although minor and low, were correlations in subtests Grouping of concepts, Relationship between verbal and non-verbal information and Intention coding. No statistically significant differences between the correlations were found; correlation pairs were averaged by the Z-transform. Results are shown in Table 3. The correlations varied from rVN x CI = -0.076 to rGS x dca = 0.711. The weighted average correlation coefficient was r = 0.398. On average, 15.84 % of individual differences in any pair of Ht subtests in a sample of twins brought up by mothers with high ES were due to the mutual variability of scores implied by these subtests. Upon exclusion of subtests Grouping of concepts, Relationship between verbal and nonverbal information and Coding of intent from the analysis (coefficients of correlation inter se and with other subtests), the average correlation coefficient varied from rpN x VN = 0.096 to rGS x dca = 0.711. This means that 77.14 % of insignificant correlations belong to subtests excluded from the Language competence factor. The weighted average phenotypic correlation coefficient was r = 0.548.
Genotypic correlations were calculated for almost all subtests, except for Address forms, Relationship between verbal and non-verbal information and Coding of intent (inter se and with other subtests). The correlations ranged from
= 0.137 to r,
: 0.986. The values of the weighted
average correlation (rg = 0.693) and the determination coefficient allowed us to conclude that an average of 48.02 % of differences in the studied pairs of linguistic characteristics can be explained by shared genetic factors. Exclusion of the genetic correlations between Ht subtests and Grouping of concepts subtest from the analysis revealed that the lowest correlation was rgPN x CS = 0.248 and that the weighted average correlation changed insignificantly (rg = 0.714), which gave an average of 50.98 % of dispersion of scores in some pairs of language competence characteristics, explained by shared genetic factors.
We found 33 environmental correlations varying from r'RS x SRW = -0.953 to r'DCA x CS = 0.894. The weighted average correlation was r' = 0.234, and the average variance of individual differences between any selected pair of characteristics was 5.48 %, explaining mutual variability of scores. Exclusion of the Grouping of concepts from the analysis brought the weighted average correlation to r' = 0.277 (determination coefficient r'2 = 0.077).
g
1
1
+
X CS
To get an idea of the interplay of phenotypic, genotypic and environmental correlations between language competencies of twins from the sample with high maternal ES, we analyzed the weighted averages available for cases with three types of correlations. The weighted average coefficients of phenotypic and genotypic correlation were r = 0.468 and rg = 0.626, respectively, determination coefficients — r2 = 0.219 and rg2 = 0.392, respectively.
Although environment only marginally influences phenotypic associations between various linguistic characteristics, the nature of these environmental factors is yet to be uncovered. These can be general family factors that, in addition to the genetic factor, result in emergence of the observed associations between linguistic characteristics. However, each language skill may be affected individually, and these effects may ultimately actualize the genetic factor shared by different characteristics, which can also foster phenotypic correlations between Ht subtests.
The following method of analysis gives a provisional answer to the question whether the detected contribution of environmental factors to the structure of the associations between the subtests is mainly based on general environmental influences or some individual environmental factors [40]. The method is based on comparing intra-individual and intra-pair
subtests cross-correlations only in the sample of MZ twins. Intra-individual correlations are explained by shared genes, shared environment and similarity of individual environmental influences. Intra-pair correlations can be explained by shared genes and shared environment only, since the individual environmental influences exercised on each twin are different. If intra-individual cross-correlations are significantly stronger than intra-pair correlations, phenotypic correlations between different subtests will result from individual environmental influences. Otherwise, phenotypic correlations will be the result of influence of the same factors related to the family environment.
Table 4 shows averaged intra-individual and intra-pair cross-correlations covering two samples of MZ twins (split-twin samples). No significant differences between these types of correlations were found.
Values of phenotypic correlations between Ht subtests calculated for the sample of twins with high maternal ES were higher than those obtained from the sample with medium maternal ES. Two split-twin subsamples that included one of the twins from MZ and DZ pairs (n1 = 28, n2 = 28) showed many insignificant correlations. In many cases, statistically significant correlations in one subsample could not be reproduced in another. However, no significant differences
Table 2. ANOVA results, two subsamples of twins (one twin In the first subsample, the other In the second subsample)
Effect of factors and their interactions Zygosity Educational status Zygosity x educational status
F emp p F p F emp p
GS1 1.991 0.163 2.264 0.137 0.139 0.710
gs2 1.914 0.171 3.555 0.064 1.701 0.197
SR1 0.776 0.382 3.290 0.074 0.036 0.851
SR2 0.436 0.511 4.750 0.033 0.003 0.954
PN1 0.004 0.949 1.606 0.208 0.456 0.502
pn2 0.639 0.427 0.329 0.568 0.625 0.432
SRW1 0.393 0.533 11.811 0.001 3.173 0.080
srw2 0.281 0.598 11.521 0.001 0.342 0.561
DCA1 1.224 0.273 5.153 0.027 0.627 0.431
dca2 1.175 0.282 3.519 0.065 0.052 0.821
cis1 0.101 0.751 10.886 0.002 0.181 0.672
CIS2 0.014 0.907 4.105 0.047 1.054 0.309
CS1 0.907 0.345 4.707 0.034 0.877 0.353
CS2 0.221 0.640 5.291 0.025 0.029 0.865
CA1 6.337 0.014 1.721 0.194 2.484 0.120
CA2 0.002 0.963 0.000 0.985 0.482 0.490
GC1 0.941 0.336 2.239 0.140 0.787 0.378
GC2 0.016 0.900 3.450 0.068 3.041 0.086
AF1 0.683 0.412 11.634 0.001 0.118 0.733
af2 0.300 0.586 5.734 0.012 1.927 0.170
VNI1 0.459 0.501 2.366 0.129 0.687 0.410
vni2 2.047 0.157 0.048 0.828 0.655 0.421
CI1 0.064 0.801 0.554 0.460 2.739 0.103
CI2 0.452 0.504 1.297 0.259 1.456 0.232
TM1 0.150 0.700 14.043 0.000 1.061 0.307
TM2 0.325 0.571 9.986 0.002 0.427 0.516
Final score1 1.456 0.232 10.074 0.002 0.021 0.884
Final score2 0.986 0.324 11.010 0.002 0.748 0.390
Note. Subscript 1 — subsample 1 consisting of first members of twin pairs; subscript 2 — subsample 2 consisting of second members of twin pairs; Femp — empirical value of the F-test; p - the exact level of statistical significance.
Table 3. Averaged phenotypic, genotypic and environmental correlations, subtests of the Heidelberg test, samples of twins from families with high and medium mothers' ES
Subtests GS SR PN SRW DCA CIS CS CA GC AF VNI CI TM
0.549**** 0.283 0.509**** 0.711**** 0.571**** 0.544**** 0.468*** 0.311 0.421** 0.328* 0.167 0.663****
GS 0.896 0.374 0.428 0.757 0.829 0.760 0.911 0.137 - - - 0.800
0.232 0.225 0.818 0.755 0.655 0.453 - 0.396 - - - 0.670
0.487** 0.432** 0.333* 0.585**** 0.515**** 0.573**** 0.456*** 0.291 0.222 0.113 0.248 0.555****
SR 0.542 0.616 0.596 0.702 0.986 0.928 0.905 0.976 - - - 0.763
0.601 -0.048 -0.953 0.250 - 0.380 - -0.672 - - - -0.314
0.323 0.294 0.207 0.387* 0.345* 0.460*** 0.501*** 0.181 0.096 0.083 0.093 0.353*
PN 0.718 0.695 0.380 0.433 0.248 0.483 0.604 0.556 - - - 0.293
- - -0.236 0.305 0.534 0.496 - -0.132 - - - 0.509
0.140 0.170 0.342 0.527**** 0.594**** 0.348* 0.370* 0.364* 0.473*** 0.255 0.237 0.413**
SRW - - - 0.925 0.714 0.664 0.849 0.209 - - - 0.549
- - - -0.646 - 0.096 - 0.729 - - - -0.109
0.369 0.410* 0.427* 0.225 0.557**** 0.544**** 0.489*** 0.309 0.471*** 0.288 0.219 0.545****
DCA 0.214 0.347 0.717 - 0.735 0.865 0.909 0.513 - - - 0.577
0.479 0.448 - - 0.894 0.356 - 0.148 - - - 0.473
0.501** 0.511** 0.295 0.208 0.087 0.533**** 0.538**** 0.338* 0.290 0.109 0.192 0.558****
CIS 0.881 0.716 0.576 - 0.002 0.907 0.855 0.284 - - - 0.620
- - - - - 0.583 - 0.397 - - - -
0.396* 0.355 0.508** 0.107 0.346 0.556*** 0.358* 0.141 0.245 0.204 0.167 0.395*
CS 0.655 0.715 0.767 - 0.432 0.511 0.932 0.944 - - - 0.365
- - - - - - - -0.198 - - - 0.557
0.326 0.431* 0.209 0.120 0.243 0.120 0.275 0.425** 0.232 0.221 0.273 0.513****
CA 0.496 0.402 0.783 - 0.352 0.476 0.974 0.391 - - - 0.727
0.198 0.464 - - 0.172 - - - - - - -
-0.072 -0.072 -0.170 0.330 -0.291 -0.040 -0.238 -0.032 0.170 -0.003 0.191 0.236
GC -0.164 -0.274 -0.550 - -0.283 -0.165 -0.679 0.021 - - - 0.414
-0.035 -0.027 - - -0.303 - - -0.055 - - - 0.074
0.516*** 0.373* 0.533*** 0.289 0.386* 0.517*** 0.477** 0.191 -0.058 0.127 -0.076 0.292
AF 0.682 0.710 0.646 - 0.560 0.611 0.478 0.814 -0.136 - - -
- - - - - - - - - - - -
-0.044 -0.077 -0.122 -0.143 0.096 0.015 0.109 0.034 -0.273 -0.040 0.139 0.317*
VNI - - - - - - - - - - - -
- - - - - - - - - - - -
0.346 0.436* 0.400* 0.222 0.348 0.209 0.396* 0.413* -0.063 0.365 0.466* 0.156
CI 0.359 0.715 0.782 - 0.435 0.605 0.689 0.450 0.212 0.657 - -
0.359 0.373 - - 0.324 - - 0.414 -0.130 - - -
0.574*** 0.240 0.426* 0.211 0.092 0.571*** 0.290 0.171 0.002 0.241 -0.122 0.164
TM 0.932 0.396 0.604 - 0.297 0.581 0.648 0.226 -0.055 0.811 - 0.509
- - - - - - - - - - - -
Note. Top of the table — data on twins from families with high maternal ES, lower part of the table — data on the sample from families with medium maternal ES. First line In each cell — phenotypic correlations, second line - genotypic correlations, third lines — environmental correlations. Dash means correlations could not be calculated. Hereinafter, levels of statistical significance: * — p < 0.05; ** — p < 0.01; *** — p < 0.005; **** — p < 0.001.
between the correlations were found, they can be averaged using Z-transform. Table 3 contains the results.
The correlations ranged from r
= -0.291 to
rGS x TM = 0.574. The weighted average for all correlations was r = 0.313. Thus, individual differences in the pairs of subtests considered, which averagely equal 9.80 %, find their explanation in the mutual variability of the relevant scores. With the Grouping of concepts, Relationship between verbal and non-verbal information and Coding of intent subtests excluded from the analysis - they accounted for 47.37 % of all statistically insignificant correlation coefficients, — the lowest correlation
was rDCA x CS = 0.087. Judging by the average weighted phenotypic correlation r = 0.356, the dispersion of subtests scores, which averages at 12.67 %, can be explained by their mutual variation.
We calculated 70.51 % of genetic correlations from the possible number of cases; the correlations varied over a wide range from rgFS x CC = -0.679 to rgFS x FW = 0.974. If the analysis includes only cases of associations between characteristics of language competence, then the lowest genetic correlation rgDCA x CS will be 0.002. The average weighted coefficient of genetic correlation rg will then equal 0.625. The mutual variability
of pairs of different language competence characteristics can be explained up to 39.06 % by of the influence of genetic factors shared by the considered pairs of subtests.
In order to conduct a comparative analysis of groups of twins raised by mothers with medium and high ES, we identified 28 cases of associations between the characteristics related to the Language competence factor. For them, phenotypic and genotypic correlations in both samples were calculated. In the group of twins from families with high maternal ES the average weighted phenotypic correlation r was 0.499, genotypic correlation rg was 0.706 and the determination coefficients were r2 = 0.249 and rg2 = 0.498, respectively. In the group of twins from medium ES families, the weighted average phenotypic (r = 0.351) and genotypic (rg = 0.559) correlations were lower (determination coefficients r2 = 0.123 and rg2 = 0.312, respectively).
We calculated environmental correlations only for 19.23 % of all possible cases. Most of them, as a rule, were characterized by negative and low correlations of subtests Grouping of concepts and Coding of intent (inter se and with other subtests). Unfortunately, we were unable to elicit sufficient number of cases of environmental correlations in order to either perform a generalized analysis or to compare environmental correlations for the same cases obtained in samples from high and medium ES groups.
DISCUSSION
Children from families with high maternal ES score better in most language subtests than twins whose mothers have a
medium ES. The differences are independent of the zygosity status, which allows uniting MZ and DZ samples for the purpose of studying the structure of phenotypic correlations. The differences in the two split-twin samples, each of which includes one twin from the pair, were reproduced in 7 subtests concerning grammar, morphology, meanings of sentences and words, and working with a text. The result agrees with the data obtained by foreign [8, 10] and Russian [11] researchers.
We have found correlations between ES and the survey data pointing out to some peculiarities in the upbringing of twins. As a whole, the results are consistent with the data obtained from the studies indicating a connection between high ES of parents and their desire to create a rich development-oriented environment for children [5, 12-14]. The present research shows that the same trend is typical for families raising children of primary school age. The results suggest that mother's ES reflects the degree of her subjective activity aimed at using a more individualized approach to the upbringing of each twin. Mothers with high ES seek to create a development-encouraging environment that allows overcoming the excessively close relationship between the twins and helps each child to become an individual. The problem of individualization is one of the most important problems of twin development [19].
It has been found that the structure of associations between Ht subtests changes depending on the maternal ES. Twins from the high ES subgroup have shown relatively strong phenotypic correlations between 10 subtests mainly related to grammar, morphology, sentence meanings and work with a text. These subtests measure language skills related to operating language constructs and not to characteristics associated with thinking or using language for the pragmatic purposes
Table 4. Averaged intra-individual and intra-pair cross-correlations. MZ twins, high ES of mothers
Subtests SR PN SRW DCA CIS CS CA GC AF VNI CI TM
GS 0.714*** 0.214 0.652*** 0.700*** 0.663*** 0.634** 0.423 0.136 0.408 0.425 0.266 0.697***
0.755**** 0.203 0.542* 0.532* 0.663*** 0.650*** 0.618** 0.147 0.387 0.294 0.156 0.727****
SR 0.405 0.505* 0.697*** 0.699*** 0.668*** 0.573* 0.238 0.250 0.398 0.375 0.714***
0.455 0.537* 0.636** 0.754**** 0.731**** 0.660*** 0.329 0.313 0.083 0.266 0.725****
PN 0.099 0.324 0.429 0.500* 0.546* 0.004 -0.167 0.086 0.221 0.403
0.157 0.366 0.366 0.486* 0.366 0.136 -0.053 0.060 0.254 0.371
SRW 0.715**** 0.568* 0.575* 0.368 0.323 0.489* 0.443 0.200 0.421
0.694*** 0.617** 0.618** 0.609** 0.272 0.431 0.595* 0.129 0.463
DCA 0.656*** 0.780**** 0.455 0.164 0.398 0.370 0.327 0.424
0.641*** 0.746**** 0.666*** 0.326 0.369 0.313 0.238 0.544*
CIS 0.627** 0.616** 0.338 0.143 0.268 0.207 0.669***
0.747**** 0.576* 0.159 0.224 0.472 0.489* 0.597*
CS 0.447 0.016 0.369 0.366 0.282 0.505
0.612** 0.345 0.130 0.281 0.355 0.551*
CA 0.303 0.126 0.225 0.247 0.616**
0.111 0.276 0.397 0.301 0.503*
GC 0.161 -0.023 0.076 0.288
0.242 0.341 0.106 0.134
AF 0.204 -0.313 0.220
0.273 -0.103 0.294
VNI 0.220 0.371
-0.002 0.168
CI 0.203
0.233
Note. Top of each cell contains averaged intra-individual cross-correlations, bottom — averaged intra-pair cross-correlation.
of communication. Based on the correlations reproduced in both groups of twins (split-twin groups, one of the pair in each group), we have identified a common linguistic factor called Language competence. On average, 30.03 % of individual differences in the characteristics included into the Language competence factor can be explained by the mutual variability of these characteristics. In the medium ES subgroup the subtests correlations were noticeably lower. A significant percentage of insignificant correlations belong to the associations that subtests excluded from the Language competence factor had inter se and with other subtests. On average, only 12.67 % of individual differences in the characteristics included into the Language competence factor can be explained by the mutual variability of these characteristics. These estimates are approximate, obtained in the generalized analysis of correlation matrices and by calculation of weighted average correlations. With a bigger sample size, it would be possible to use other, more advanced methods of statistical analysis. Thus, the improvement of language skills possessed by children from families with high maternal ES is associated with the increased structural connection between relevant linguistic characteristics.
The results allow deducing a provisional answer to the question whether the nature of phenotypic associations between linguistic characteristics observed in children varies depending on the ES of their mothers. Since not all the pairs of subtests allowed calculation of genotypic correlations, the comparative analysis could only be performed in 28 cases, regarding the structure of genetic correlations in the subgroups of twins raised by mothers with different ES. While in the high ES sample phenotypic differences in the considered Language competence aspects could for the average of about 24.90 % be explained by the mutual variation of these characteristics, in the medium ES group the mutual phenotypic variability for the same subtests describes about 12.32 % of the scores. At that, the first sample had the observed associations explained by the influence of shared genetic factors for the average of about 49.84 %, and in the second sample the contribution of the shared genetic factors to the explanation of the obtained phenotypic correlations was only 31.25 %. There is a reason to believe that maternal ES determines more than just differences in the dynamics of genetic and environmental contributions to the variability of language skills, which has been reported by researchers both abroad [33] and in Russia [32]. This factor determines the differences in the level of dependence of structural interconnectedness between language competencies on the genetic factors shared by them. Following the assumptions of Bronfenbrenner and Ceci's bioecological model and Scarr's ideas [21, 22], it can be assumed that individualized upbringing strategies exercised by mothers with high ES, effected through creation of an environment stimulating language skills development, triggers actualization of the genetic potential underlying the shared linguistic factor. The psychogenetic approach based on biometric statistics does not allow identifying these genes. The present study supports the need for discovery of these genes using methods of molecular genetics.
The results allow conducting a comparative analysis of the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the structure of the associations between different language competencies only for the subgroup of twins whose mothers have a high ES. On average, in 33 cases for which phenotypic,
genotypic and environmental correlations were calculated, a fifth (21.90 %) of individual differences in correlated subtests finds explanation in their mutual variation, with an average of 39.19 % of their mutual variability attributable to the effect of a shared genetic factor. Environmental influences explain only about 7.67 %.
Comparison of intra-individual and intra-pair cross-correlations between different subtests done in the MZ sample allowed tomake a preliminary conclusion: the insignificant contribution of the environmental component of phenotypic correlations between Ht subtests can be explained by the influence of the family environment. In this light and bearing in mind the tendency to individualize the development-oriented environment shown by mothers with high ES, it is necessary to explain negative environmental correlations witnessed in some cases. For example, the highest negative environment correlation r' = -0.953 is observed between the subtests Word formation and Imitation of grammatical structures. With the moderate positive phenotypic correlation (r = 0.333) and significant positive genetic correlation (rg = 0.596), environmental influences yield directly opposite results in the development of both abilities. Slight differences between intra-individual and intra-pair cross-correlations between these subtests indicate that we are dealing with the general environment influences that lead to diametrically opposite phenotypic indicators of these abilities. At the same time, a moderate phenotypic relationship remains between the scores of the two considered subtests.
Therefore, we state that individualization of educational influences in families where mothers have high ES boosts children's language skills development, promotes growth of structural cohesion of the characteristics of language competence, ups the role of the genotypic factor and lowers the influence of the family environment in explanations of the nature of this structural cohesion.
CONCLUSIONS
High ES of a mother is a factor in boosting child's language competence that works through creation of a more individualized development-oriented environment (in the context of our twin research: for each twin in a pair, regardless of zygosity status). Higher educational status of mothers means better structural integrity of the aspects of language competence, which is substantially backed by the increased contribution of genetic factors governing language skills. At that, general, mostly family-related, environmental factors make a significantly smaller contribution to the close associations between the aspects of the child's language competence.
The aim of our pilot study was achieved. The conclusions drawn are preliminary. There is a need to conduct studies with larger sample sizes, compiled on the basis of more accurate criteria that take into account all nuances of the family's socioeconomic status. To overcome the limitations inherent in the twin method, it is necessary to conduct research using other psychogenetic methods. Such an approach would allow a generalized analysis that incorporates data on different types of pairs of relatives, as well as application of some more complex genetic and mathematical methods. The results of research efforts as described above would form the basis for molecular genetic studies of etiology of individual differences in language competencies.
References
1. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002; 53: 371-99. PubMed PMID: 11752490.
2. Chernov DN. Sotsiokul'turnaya obuslovlennost' stanovleniya yazykovoy kompetentsii. Moscow: TEZAURUS; 2014. 151 p. Russian.
3. Arriaga RI, Fenson L, Cronan T, Pethick SJ. Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998; 19 (2): 209-23.
4. Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE, Pitcairn DN, et al. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999 Dec; 42 (6): 1432-43. PubMed PMID: 10599625.
5. Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998; 19 (4): 603-29.
6. Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cogn Psychol. 2010 Dec; 61 (4): 343-65. PubMed PMID: 20832781.
7. Morisset C, Barnard K, Greenberg M, Booth C, Spieker S. Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Dev Psychopathol. 1990; 2 (2): 127-49.
8. Jordan N, Huttenlocher J, Levine S. Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. Dev Psychol. 1992; 28 (4): 644-53.
9. Lawrence VW. Middle- and working-class Black and White children's speech during a picture-labeling task. J Genet Psychol. 1997 Jun; 158 (2): 226-40. PubMed PMID: 9168591.
10. Snow C. Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harv Educ Rev. 1983 Summer; 53 (2): 165-89.
11. Chernov DN. [Interrelation between parent relation type and speech development in primary schoolers from families with different socioeconomic status]. Psikhologicheskie Issledovaniya [serial on the Internet]. 2011 [cited 2017 Jun 26]; (1): article 2 [about 7 p.]. Available from: http://psystudy.ru/index.php/ num/2011n1-15/430-chernov15.html Russian.
12. Camp BW, Cunningham M, Berman S. Relationship between the cognitive environment and vocabulary development during the second year of life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010 Oct; 164 (10): 950-6. PubMed PMID: 20921353.
13. Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 2003 Sep-Oct; 74 (5): 1368-78. PubMed PMID: 14552403.
14. Hoff-Ginsberg E. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 1991 Aug; 62 (4): 782-96. PubMed PMID: 1935343.
15. Schady N. Parents' education, mothers' vocabulary, and cognitive development in early childhood: longitudinal evidence from Ecuador. Am J Public Health. 2011 Dec; 101 (12): 2299307. PubMed PMID: 22021308.
16. Son SH, Morrison FJ. The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev Psychol. 2010 Sep; 46 (5): 110318. PubMed PMID: 20822226.
17. Pancsofar N, Vernon-Feagans L; The Family Life Project Investigators. Fathers' early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Child Res Q. 2010 Oct; 25 (4): 450-63. PubMed PMID: 21057648.
18. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Shannon JD, Cabrera NJ, Lamb ME. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004 Nov-Dec; 75 (6): 1806-20. PubMed PMID: 15566381.
19. Ravich-Shcherbo IV, Maryutina TM, Grigorenko EL. Psikhogenetika. Moscow: Aspekt Press; 1999. 447 p. Russian.
20. Chernov DN. [Psychogenetic studies of speech and language abilities: The short review and studying prospects]. Sovremennaya zarubezhnaya psikhologiya [serial on the Internet]. 2014 [cited 2017 Jun 26]; 3 (2): 5-17. Available from: http://psyjournals.ru/
jmfp/2014/n2/70094.shtml Russian.
21. Bronfenbrenner U, Ceci SJ. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bio-ecological model. Psychol Rev. 1994 Oct; 101 (4): 568-86. PubMed PMID: 7984707.
22. Scarr S. Developmental theories for the 1990s: development and individual differences. Child Dev. 1992 Feb; 63 (1): 1-19. PubMed PMID: 1343618.
23. Rhemtulla M, Tucker-Drob EM. Gene-by-socioeconomic status interaction on school readiness. Behav Genet. 2012 Jul; 42 (4): 549-58. PubMed PMID: 22350185.
24. Tucker-Drob EM, Rhemtulla M, Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Fask D. Emergence of a gene x socioeconomic status interaction on infant mental ability between 10 months and 2 years. Psychol Sci. 2011 Jan; 22 (1): 125-33. PubMed PMID: 21169524.
25. Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D'Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci. 2003 Nov; 14 (6): 623-8. PubMed PMID: 14629696.
26. Schwartz JA. Socioeconomic status as a moderator of the genetic and shared environmental influence on verbal IQ: A multilevel behavioral genetic approach. Intelligence. 2015 Sep-Oct; 52: 80-9.
27. Hart SA, Petrill SA, Deckard KD, Thompson LA. SES and CHAOS as environmental mediators of cognitive ability: A longitudinal genetic analysis. Intelligence. 2007 May-Jun; 35 (3): 233-42.
28. Tucker-Drob EM, Bates TC. Large cross-national differences in gene x socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2016 Feb; 27 (2): 138-49. PubMed PMID: 26671911.
29. Petrill SA, Pike A, Price T, Plomin R. Chaos in the home and socioeconomic status are associated with cognitive development in early childhood: Environmental mediators identified in a genetic design. Intelligence. 2004 Sep-Oct; 32 (5): 445-60.
30. Stromswold K. Biological and psychosocial factors affect linguistic and cognitive development differently: A twin study. In: Bamman D, Magnitskaia T, Zaller C, editors. BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development; 2005 Nov 4-6; Boston, USA. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; 2006. p. 595-606.
31. Hart SA, Soden B, Johnson W, Schatschneider C, Taylor J. Expanding the environment: gene x school-level SES interaction on reading comprehension. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013 Oct; 54 (10): 1047-55. PubMed PMID: 23725549.
32. Ravich-Shcherbo IV, Chernov DN. Psikhogeneticheskoe issledovanie individual'nykh osobennostey rechi v mladshem shkol'nom vozraste. Zhurnal prikladnoy psikhologii. 2005; (1): 21-9. Russian.
33. Rowe DC, Jacobson KC, Van den Oord EJ. Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as moderator. Child Dev. 1999 Sep-Oct; 70 (5): 1151-62. PubMed PMID: 10546338.
34. Bates TC, Lewis GJ, Weiss A. Childhood socioeconomic status amplifies genetic effects on adult intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2013 Oct; 24 (10): 2111-6. PubMed PMID: 24002887.
35. Kremen WS, Jacobson KC, Xian H, Eisen SA, Waterman B, Toomey R, et al. Heritability of word recognition in middle-aged men varies as a function of parental education. Behav Genet. 2005 Jul; 35 (4): 417-33. PubMed PMID: 15971023.
36. Chernov DN. [Psychogenetic study of language competence's structure]. Psikhologicheskie Issledovaniya [serial on the Internet]. 2017 [cited 2017 Jun 26]; 10 (52): article 10 [about 7 p.]. Available from: http://psystudy.ru/index.php/num/2017v10n52/1409-chernov52.html Russian.
37. Mikhaylova NB. Adaptirovannyy variant Geydel'bergskogo testa rechevogo razvitiya detey. Psikhologicheskiy zhurnal. 1990; 11 (6): 105-12. Russian.
38. Maryutina TM. [Intermediate Phenotypes of intelligence in the context of genetic psychophysiology]. Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshey shkoly ekonomiki. 2007; 4 (2): 22-47. Russian.
39. Searle SR. Phenotypic, genetic and environmental correlation. Biometrics. 1961 Sep; 17 (3): 474-80.
40. Scourfield J, Martin N, Eley TC, McGuffin P. The genetic relationship between social cognition and conduct problems. Behav Genet. 2004 Jul; 34 (4): 377-83. PubMed PMID: 15082935.
Литература
1. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002; 53: 371-99. PubMed PMID: 11752490.
2. Чернов Д. Н. Социокультурная обусловленность становления языковой компетенции. М.: ТЕЗАУРУС; 2014. 151 с.
3. Arriaga RI, Fenson L, Cronan T, Pethick SJ. Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998; 19 (2): 209-23.
4. Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Paradise JL, Feldman HM, Janosky JE, Pitcairn DN, et al. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999 Dec; 42 (6): 1432-43. PubMed PMID: 10599625.
5. Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998; 19 (4): 603-29.
6. Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cogn Psychol. 2010 Dec; 61 (4): 343-65. PubMed PMID: 20832781.
7. Morisset C, Barnard K, Greenberg M, Booth C, Spieker S. Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Dev Psychopathol. 1990; 2 (2): 127-49.
8. Jordan N, Huttenlocher J, Levine S. Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. Dev Psychol. 1992; 28 (4): 644-53.
9. Lawrence VW. Middle- and working-class Black and White children's speech during a picture-labeling task. J Genet Psychol. 1997 Jun; 158 (2): 226-40. PubMed PMID: 9168591.
10. Snow C. Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harv Educ Rev. 1983 Summer; 53 (2): 165-89.
11. Чернов Д. Н. Взаимосвязь типа родительского отношения и речевого развития младших школьников в семьях с разным социоэкономическим статусом. Психологические исследования: электрон. науч. журн. [Интернет]. 2011 [дата обращения: 26 июня 2017 г.]; (1): статья № 2 [примерно 7 с.]. Доступно по: http://psystudy.ru/index.php/num/2011n1-15/430-chernov15.html
12. Camp BW, Cunningham M, Berman S. Relationship between the cognitive environment and vocabulary development during the second year of life. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010 Oct; 164 (10): 950-6. PubMed PMID: 20921353.
13. Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 2003 Sep-Oct; 74 (5): 1368-78. PubMed PMID: 14552403.
14. Hoff-Ginsberg E. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 1991 Aug; 62 (4): 782-96. PubMed PMID: 1935343.
15. Schady N. Parents' education, mothers' vocabulary, and cognitive development in early childhood: longitudinal evidence from Ecuador. Am J Public Health. 2011 Dec; 101 (12): 2299307. PubMed PMID: 22021308.
16. Son SH, Morrison FJ. The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev Psychol. 2010 Sep; 46 (5): 110318. PubMed PMID: 20822226.
17. Pancsofar N, Vernon-Feagans L; The Family Life Project Investigators. Fathers' early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Child Res Q. 2010 Oct; 25 (4): 450-63. PubMed PMID: 21057648.
18. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Shannon JD, Cabrera NJ, Lamb ME. Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004 Nov-Dec; 75 (6): 1806-20. PubMed PMID: 15566381.
19. Равич-Щербо И. В., Марютина Т. М., Григоренко Е. Л. Психогенетика. М.: Аспект Пресс; 1999. 447 с.
20. Чернов Д. Н. Психогенетические исследования речевых и языковых способностей: краткий обзор и перспективы изучения. Современная зарубежная психология [Интернет]. 2014 [дата обращения: 26 июня 2017 г.]; 3 (2): [5-17]. Доступно по: http://psyjournals.ru/jmfp/2014/n2/70094.shtml
21. Bronfenbrenner U, Ceci SJ. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bio-ecological model. Psychol Rev. 1994 Oct; 101 (4): 568-86. PubMed PMID: 7984707.
22. Scarr S. Developmental theories for the 1990s: development and individual differences. Child Dev. 1992 Feb; 63 (1): 1-19. PubMed PMID: 1343618.
23. Rhemtulla M, Tucker-Drob EM. Gene-by-socioeconomic status interaction on school readiness. Behav Genet. 2012 Jul; 42 (4): 549-58. PubMed PMID: 22350185.
24. Tucker-Drob EM, Rhemtulla M, Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Fask D. Emergence of a gene x socioeconomic status interaction on infant mental ability between 10 months and 2 years. Psychol Sci. 2011 Jan; 22 (1): 125-33. PubMed PMID: 21169524.
25. Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D'Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci. 2003 Nov; 14 (6): 623-8. PubMed PMID: 14629696.
26. Schwartz JA. Socioeconomic status as a moderator of the genetic and shared environmental influence on verbal IQ: A multilevel behavioral genetic approach. Intelligence. 2015 Sep-Oct; 52: 80-9.
27. Hart SA, Petrill SA, Deckard KD, Thompson LA. SES and CHAOS as environmental mediators of cognitive ability: A longitudinal genetic analysis. Intelligence. 2007 May-Jun; 35 (3): 233-42.
28. Tucker-Drob EM, Bates TC. Large cross-national differences in gene x socioeconomic status interaction on intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2016 Feb; 27 (2): 138-49. PubMed PMID: 26671911.
29. Petrill SA, Pike A, Price T, Plomin R. Chaos in the home and socioeconomic status are associated with cognitive development in early childhood: Environmental mediators identified in a genetic design. Intelligence. 2004 Sep-Oct; 32 (5): 445-60.
30. Stromswold K. Biological and psychosocial factors affect linguistic and cognitive development differently: A twin study. In: Bamman D, Magnitskaia T, Zaller C, editors. BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development; 2005 Nov 4-6; Boston, USA. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; 2006. p. 595-606.
31. Hart SA, Soden B, Johnson W, Schatschneider C, Taylor J. Expanding the environment: gene x school-level SES interaction on reading comprehension. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013 Oct; 54 (10): 1047-55. PubMed PMID: 23725549.
32. Равич-Щербо И. В., Чернов Д. Н. Психогенетическое исследование индивидуальных особенностей речи в младшем школьном возрасте. Журн. приклад. психол. 2005; (1): 21-9.
33. Rowe DC, Jacobson KC, Van den Oord EJ. Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as moderator. Child Dev. 1999 Sep-Oct; 70 (5): 1151-62. PubMed PMID: 10546338.
34. Bates TC, Lewis GJ, Weiss A. Childhood socioeconomic status amplifies genetic effects on adult intelligence. Psychol Sci. 2013 Oct; 24 (10): 2111-6. PubMed PMID: 24002887.
35. Kremen WS, Jacobson KC, Xian H, Eisen SA, Waterman B, Toomey R, et al. Heritability of word recognition in middle-aged men varies as a function of parental education. Behav Genet. 2005 Jul; 35 (4): 417-33. PubMed PMID: 15971023.
36. Чернов Д. Н. Психогенетическое исследование структуры языковой компетенции. Психологические исследования: электрон. науч. журн. [Интернет]. 2017 [дата обращения: 26 июня 2017 г.]; 10 (52): статья № 10 [примерно 7 с.]. Доступно по: http://psystudy.ru/index.php/num/2017v10n52/1409-chernov52.html
37. Михайлова Н. Б. Адаптированный вариант Гейдельбергского теста речевого развития детей. Психол. журн. 1990; 11 (6): 105-12.
38. Марютина Т. М. Промежуточные фенотипы интеллекта в контексте генетической психофизиологии. Психология. Журн. Высшей школы экономики. 2007; 4 (2): 22-47.
39. Searle SR. Phenotypic, genetic and environmental correlation. Biometrics. 1961 Sep; 17 (3): 474-80.
40. Scourfield J, Martin N, Eley TC, McGuffin P. The genetic relationship between social cognition and conduct problems. Behav Genet. 2004 Jul; 34 (4): 377-83. PubMed PMID: 15082935.