Научная статья на тему 'STUDYING SPATIAL ORIENTATION AND CONSTRUCTIVE PRAXIS DISORDERS IN NORMALLY DEVELOPING AND MENTALLY RETARDED CHILDREN AGED 8-11 YEARS'

STUDYING SPATIAL ORIENTATION AND CONSTRUCTIVE PRAXIS DISORDERS IN NORMALLY DEVELOPING AND MENTALLY RETARDED CHILDREN AGED 8-11 YEARS Текст научной статьи по специальности «Науки о здоровье»

CC BY
93
18
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Журнал
Science for Education Today
Область наук
Ключевые слова
INTERHEMISPHERIC INTERACTION / BRAIN FUNCTIONS / SPATIAL ORIENTATION DISORDERS / INTELLECTUAL DISABILITIES / CONSTRUCTIVE PRAXIS / MENTAL DEVELOPMENT OF CHILDREN / PRIMARY SCHOOL

Аннотация научной статьи по наукам о здоровье, автор научной работы — Azatyan Tereza Yurevna, Piskun Olga Yuryevna

Introduction. The ability to orientate oneself in space is one of the basic requirements for the harmonious development of a person. The problem of human orientation in space is very complex. It includes both ideas about the size and shape of objects and the ability to distinguish the location of objects in space, and understanding different spatial relationships. The concept of spatial orientation includes the evaluation of distances, sizes, shapes, mutual position of objects and their position relative to the orienting object. Spatial orientation is significantly impaired in mentally handicapped children, since the impairment of mental development not only limits their motor activity, but also negatively affects the development of time representations, musculoskeletal functions and leads to a decrease in compensatory processes. The purpose of this study is to identify spatial orientation and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years. Materials and Methods. The methodological basis of this research investigation was scholarly literature devoted to studying nervous functions in children with mental development disorders and interhemispheric asymmetry of the brain, regulation of these functions, identifying difficulties, modeling the system of work with these children and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years (Boguslavskaya, Miroshnichenko, 2019, Maryutina, Yermolaev 2001, Reuter-Lorenz and others, 2000, Azatyan2022). The study was conducted in the research laboratory of the Faculty of Special and Inclusive Education at the Armenian State Pedagogical University after Kh. Abovyan and research laboratory in Armenian state institute of physical culture and sport. A total of 131 children aged between 8 and 11 years participated in the study, including 73 healthy schoolchildren and 58 children with mild mental retardation. The degree of intellectual disability was assessed based on medical indications, degree of social adjustment, level of intellectual functioning, and learning outcomes in special education classes. Each category of participants was divided into 2 age groups: 8-9 years old and 10-11 years old. Results. The article discusses the main issues of studies of spatial orientation and constructive praxis in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged between 8 and 11 years, provides a scientific rationale for the problem, taking into account previous research investigations. However, the problem of mentally retarded and normally developing children in this age group remains virtually unexplored. Analysis of research findings allows the authors to identify and summarize the following important points: - mentally handicapped primary school children lag behind their healthy peers in all indicators examined; - 8- and 9-year-olds have less developed practical spatial orientation skills than their older peers; - Intellectually disabled peers have underdeveloped spatial orientation skills and constructive practice, making it difficult to understand spatial relationships between objects. The experimental study of task performance revealed interesting data, which will serve as a basis for developing the necessary means, methods and conditions for the development of elementary practical orientation in mentally disabled primary schoolchildren. The authors have uncovered violations of spatial orientation and constructive praxis in normally developing and intellectually disabled children aged 8-11 years that highlight the limited and incomplete verbalization of spatial relationships of objects in intellectually disabled children. The listed errors in general are reduced to the use by mentally retarded schoolchildren of imprecise, vague spatial characteristics instead of more differentiated ones. Conclusions. Thus, the results of the conducted experimental research testify to the insufficient development of the elementary practical orientation and understanding of the spatial relationships of objects in mentally retarded children of primary school age.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «STUDYING SPATIAL ORIENTATION AND CONSTRUCTIVE PRAXIS DISORDERS IN NORMALLY DEVELOPING AND MENTALLY RETARDED CHILDREN AGED 8-11 YEARS»

Science for Education Today

2023, vol. 13, issue 1 http://en.sciforedu.ru/

ISSN 2658-6762

DOI: 10.15293/2658-6762.2301.08

Research Full Article/ Article language: English

Studying spatial orientation and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years

Tereza Yu. Azatyan h Olga Yu. Piskun2

1 Armenian State Institute of Physical Culture and Sport, Yerevan, Republic of Armenia 2 Novosibirsk State Pedagogical University, Novosibirsk, Russian Federation

Abstract

Introduction.

The ability to orientate oneself in space is one of the basic requirements for the harmonious development of a person. The problem of human orientation in space is very complex. It includes both ideas about the size and shape of objects and the ability to distinguish the location of objects in space, and understanding different spatial relationships. The concept of spatial orientation includes the evaluation of distances, sizes, shapes, mutual position of objects and their position relative to the orienting object. Spatial orientation is significantly impaired in mentally handicapped children, since the impairment of mental development not only limits their motor activity, but also negatively affects the development of time representations, musculoskeletal functions and leads to a decrease in compensatory processes.

The purpose of this study is to identify spatial orientation and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years.

Materials and Methods.

The methodological basis of this research investigation was scholarly literature devoted to studying nervous functions in children with mental development disorders and interhemispheric asymmetry of the brain, regulation of these functions, identifying difficulties, modeling the system of work with these children and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years (Boguslavskaya, Miroshnichenko, 2019, Maryutina, Yermolaev 2001, Reuter-Lorenz and others, 2000, Azatyan2022).

The study was conducted in the research laboratory of the Faculty of Special and Inclusive Education at the Armenian State Pedagogical University after Kh. Abovyan and research laboratory in Armenian state institute of physical culture and sport.

For citation

Azatyan T. Yu., Piskun O. Yu. Studying spatial orientation and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 years. Science for Education Today, 2023, vol. 13 (1), pp. 159-174. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15293/2658-6762.2301.08

E3 Corresponding Author: Tereza Yu. Azatyan, atereza222@gmail.com © Tereza Yu. Azatyan, Olga Yu. Piskun, 2023

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

159

© ®

All rights reserved

Science for Education Today

2023, vol. 13, issue 1 http://en.sciforedu.ru/ ISSN 2658-6762

A total of 131 children aged between 8 and 11 years participated in the study, including 73 healthy schoolchildren and 58 children with mild mental retardation. The degree of intellectual disability was assessed based on medical indications, degree of social adjustment, level of intellectual functioning, and learning outcomes in special education classes. Each category of participants was divided into 2 age groups: 8-9 years old and 10-11 years old.

Results. The article discusses the main issues of studies of spatial orientation and constructive praxis in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged between 8 and 11 years, provides a scientific rationale for the problem, taking into account previous research investigations. However, the problem of mentally retarded and normally developing children in this age group remains virtually unexplored.

Analysis of research findings allows the authors to identify and summarize the following important points:

- mentally handicapped primary school children lag behind their healthy peers in all indicators examined;

- 8- and 9-year-olds have less developed practical spatial orientation skills than their older peers;

- Intellectually disabled peers have underdeveloped spatial orientation skills and constructive practice, making it difficult to understand spatial relationships between objects.

The experimental study of task performance revealed interesting data, which will serve as a basis for developing the necessary means, methods and conditions for the development ofelementary practical orientation in mentally disabled primary schoolchildren.

The authors have uncovered violations of spatial orientation and constructive praxis in normally developing and intellectually disabled children aged 8-11 years that highlight the limited and incomplete verbalization of spatial relationships of objects in intellectually disabled children. The listed errors in general are reduced to the use by mentally retarded schoolchildren of imprecise, vague spatial characteristics instead of more differentiated ones.

Conclusions. Thus, the results of the conducted experimental research testify to the insufficient development of the elementary practical orientation and understanding of the spatial relationships of objects in mentally retarded children of primary school age.

Interhemispheric interaction; Brain functions; Spatial orientation disorders; Intellectual disabilities; Constructive praxis; Mental development of children; Primary school.

Keywords

The problem of human orientation in space is quite multifaceted. It includes both ideas about the size and shape of objects and the ability to

The ability to orientate in space is one of the basic conditions for the harmonious development of a person1 [1].

Introduction

distinguish the location of objects in space, understanding of different spatial relationships [2-10]. The notion of spatial orientation includes the assessment of distances, sizes, shapes, mutual position of objects and their position relative to the orientator [11; 12].

1 Ermakov P. N., Boguslavskaya V. F. Interhemispheric functional asymmetry of preschool children with different readiness to study at school. Psychological Bulletin of the Russian State University, 1999, pp. 70-73.

Ermakov P. N., Shumakova E. R. Interhemispheric functional asymmetry and bimanual activity. Psychological Bulletin of the Russian State University, 1999, Issue 4, pp. 14-32.

The simplest forms of spatial orientation, associated with the transition to the upright position and the development of object actions, are formed as early as infancy. By the age of three a child develops a systematic mechanism of spatial orientation in which vision, kinesthesia, and static-dynamic sensations are interconnected [13].

By entering all areas of a child's interaction with reality, spatial orientation influences the development of self-awareness and personality and is an integral part of the socialisation process [14; 15].

Spatial orientation refers to "a person's understanding and determination of his or her position in relation to some material bodies that he or she has chosen", or, similarly, their position in relation to himself or herself [16; 17].

For mentally retarded children, spatial orientation is significantly hampered [18; 19] because the impairment of mental development not only limits their motor activity, but also negatively affects the development of temporal representations, musculoskeletal function and leads to a decrease in compensatory processes2.

The scientific and methodological literature [20-25] indicates the importance of developing spatial representations and practical orientation of mentally retarded children, but does not reveal the potential of their content. Therefore, the problem of correction of spatial representations and orientation disorders, as well as the development of orienting skills in the conditions of special school education are of extreme importance3.

In connection with the above, we set the goal of identifying spatial orientation and constructive praxis disorders in normally

developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11.

Methods

The methodological basis of this research investigation was scholarly works by a number of authors devoted to the study of nervous functions in children with mental development disorders and interhemispheric asymmetry of the brain, regulation of these functions, identifying difficulties, modeling the system of work with these children and constructive praxis disorders in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 8-11 (Boguslavskaya, Miroshnichenko, 2019 [1], Maryutina, Yermolaev 2001 4, Reuter-Lorenz and others, 2000 [10], Azatyan, 2022 [12]).

The research was performed in the research laboratory of the Faculty of Special and Inclusive Education at the Armenian State Pedagogical University after Kh. Abovyan.

A total of 131 children aged between 8 and 11 years participated in the study, including 73 healthy schoolchildren and 58 children with mild mental retardation.

The degree of mental retardation was assessed on the basis of medical indications, degree of social adaptation, level of intellectual functioning and mastery of the school programme during training in a special educational institution.

Each category of examinees was divided into 2 age groups: 8-9 years old and 10-11 years old.

Control group of healthy schoolchildren:

- 8-9 years old 38 children, including 20 girls and 18 boys;

2 Semenovich A. V. Neuropsychological diagnostics and correction in childhood. M.: Academy, 2002. 232 p.

3 Barkov V. A., Poleshchuk A. M. Formation of spatial orientation in schoolchildren with mental retardation.

Special Education, 2010, no. 2, pp. 46-55.

4 Maryutina M. T., Ermolaev O. Yu. Introduction to psy-chophysiology: textbook. Moscow, Psychological and Social Institute Publishing House "Flint" 2001, 400 p.

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

161

© ®

All rights reserved

- 10-11 years old 35 children including 17 girls and 18 boys;

Experimental group of children with mental retardation:

- 8-9 years - 28 children, including 11 girls and 17 boys;

- 10-11 years old 25 children, including 12 girls and 13 boys;

Children in the experimental group were selected on the basis of supporting documents with an approved diagnosis of mild mental retardation. After reviewing the results of clinical, laboratory, pedagogical and psychological examinations, a voluntary agreement was signed with family members and carers to allow the child to participate in the research.

In order to determine the level of development of elementary practical orientation: the subjects were offered the following tasks:

- show their right (left) hand;

- show the right (left) hand to the other person standing opposite;

- indicate what is on your right (left) side.

All studies were conducted separately for

children with a leading right hand and a leading left hand, as it is reasonable to assume that children will be more successful in tests aimed at their leading hand.

We first sought to find out whether children with intellectual disabilities can correctly identify their right and left hand and the person facing them, how well they can orient in space and determine what is on their right or left side, identify the right or left-hand drawer of a table, put objects into the right or left-hand pocket of a jacket, etc. Because such elementary practical orientation skills often determine a mentally

retarded child's readiness to perform the more complex social, occupational and other activities needed in everyday life.

In these experiments we widely used both the verbal designation of objects and actions, and drawings, pictures of the same objects and actions contained in the proposed task. These are first of all household items and their pictures, well known to children: a key, a table, a glass, a pencil, etc. As pupils of special (auxiliary) school often, deal not only with natural objects, but also with their images.

In addition, the graphic representation of objects and actions with them will help correctly and more quickly identify difficulties in completing the proposed tasks, as in the works of T. I. Golovina and others5 there is data indicating a lack of understanding by mentally retarded younger pupils of the spatial relationships of the depicted objects. Taking this into account, we tried to use certain groups of words and word combinations reflecting spatial relations of objects.

Before subjects started each subsequent task we inquired whether they understood the meaning of the task, and until they had a clear idea of the meaning of the task and what was required of them, they did not proceed with it.

Results

Experimental results for children with a leading right hand and a leading left hand

The first task. ("Show your right (left) hand") was aimed at determining the subjects' right and left hand orientation skills. Of the 29 normally developing right-handed children 89 years of age, 69 % of the students correctly

5 Makoedova G. V. Formation of personal qualities in children with severe speech disorders in the light of the Federal State Educational Standard. Petrozaovdsk, Publishing House. PetrGU, 2016. pp. 128-130.

Melikyan Z. A., Sokorokhodova T. A. Individual features of the functional organization of the brain of children 78 years old with mental retardation in the process of processing visual-spatial information. Human Physiology, 2002, no. 2, pp. 31-34.

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

162

© ®

All rights reserved

2023, vol. 13, issue 1 http://en.sciforedu.ru/ ISSN 2658-6762

oriented themselves, while only 47.3 % of the 19 mentally retarded right-handed children managed to do this task (Table 1).

Table 1

Results of 'right-handed' test takers on the task to show right (left) hand

Age Test takers

Normally developing children Mentally retarded children

Number Of test takers Correct Mistake Number Of test takers Correct Mistake

num % num % num % num %

8-9 29 20 69 9 41 19 9 47,3 10 52,7

10-11 28 21 75 7 25 18 10 55,5 8 44,5

Total 57 41 72 16 28 37 19 51,3 18 48,7

In the group of 10-11 year olds with no intellectual disability, there were 28 right-handed children, of whom 75 % (21 children) showed correct orientation, (25 %) 7 children made a consistent error.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

The ambidextrous were counted in the right-handed groups because their sensory asymmetry of the praxis test revealed a right-handed preference.

Results of "left-handed" subjects i

Out of a total of 73 children with normal mental development, 13 left-handed children were identified, distributed roughly equally across age groups: 7 children aged 8-9 years and 6 children aged 10-11 years. The test showed that 57 % of 8-9 year olds and 83.3 % of 10-11 year olds showed correct side preference (see Table 2).

Table 2

the task show the right (left) hand

Age Test takers

Normally developing children Mentally retarded children

Number Of test takers Correct Mistake Number Of test takers Correct Mistake

num % num % num % num %

8-9 7 4 57 3 43 7 3 43 4 57

1011 6 5 83,3 1 17,7 7 4 57 3 43

Total 13 9 68,5 4 31,5 14 26 44,8 34 58,6

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

163

© ®

All rights reserved

In the experimental group of children with mental retardation, there were seven left-handed children in each age group (14 in total). Among 8-9 year olds, 43 % and among 10-11 year olds 57 % correctly coped with the task, while orientation disorders were found in 57 % and 43 %, respectively, by age group.

As seen from these results, children in the older age group (ages 10-11) did better than children in the younger age group (ages 8-9). This trend was also observed in the group of mentally retarded children, although it was not as pronounced as in the normal group.

So, we can state that mentally retarded "right-handed" and "left-handed" children are poorly oriented in identifying right and left-handedness and lag behind compared to normally developing peers in these measures.

The second, more difficult task "Show the right (left) hand to the other person standing in front of you" caused great difficulties for mentally retarded students. Only 31.0 % of 8-11 year old pupils in special (auxiliary) schools were able to cope with this task. Many normally developing children also had some difficulty with this activity, but to a much lesser extent.

Table 3

Results of the correct performance of the task "Show the right (left) hand of the other person standing in front of you (left) hand of the other person standing opposite"

Age Trial subjects

Normally developing children Mentally retarded children

Number of test takers Correct answers % Number of test takers Correct answers %

8 years 19 13 68,4 16 3 18,6

9 years 21 15 71,4 17 4 23,6

10 years 16 11 68,8 12 6 30,0

11 years 17 14 82,3 13 5 38,5

Total 73 43 58,9 58 18 31,0

The table shows that 8- and 9-year-olds were most likely to confuse the right and left hands of the person standing in front of them. Thus, about 81.3 % of mentally retarded 8-year-olds could not cope with this task, 9-year-olds 76.5 %, and 10-11-year-olds, although they improved slightly, still remain at a low level. Compared to normally developing peers, the performance of mentally retarded children is significantly lower (Table 3). For example, while

normally developing examinees correctly identified the right and left hand of the person standing in front of them by age 10-11, the mentally retarded peers improved in the performance of this task from 18.6 % to 38.5 %, respectively, by age 8-11.

Thus, the data presented indicate a significant difficulty for mentally retarded students to mentally turn 180 degrees. This confirms the evidence in the literature about the

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

164

© ®

All rights reserved

lack of maturation of the system of backward orienting actions in children of this age.

In the following task, when asked, "What is on your left (right) side?", retarded children mostly noted large objects, although not all of them. For example, they named a cupboard, a table, chairs, a window, but they did not notice small objects such as books, a pen, pencils, a vase, etc. (although they were nearby).

Among mentally retarded children of age 8, only two children (23.5 %) handled this task satisfactorily, while among 9-year-olds only 4 (23.5 %) out of 17 children handled the task. By the age of 10-11 these figures increased slightly, but still remained low (Table 4).

Table 4

Results of the correct performance of the task "Point out what is on your right (left) on your right (left) side"

Age Trial subjects

Normally developing children Mentally retarded children

Number of test takers Correct answers % Number of test takers Correct answers %

8 years 19 15 78,9 16 2 12,5

9 years 21 18 85,7 17 4 23,5

10 years 16 12 75,0 12 5 41,7

11 years 17 13 76,5 13 5 38,5

Total 73 58 79,5 58 16 27,6

Thus, more than half of the 10-11 year olds were able to list objects on their right (or left) side. These indicators are significantly lower as compared to normally developing examinees (Table 4). This lag is observed at all ages under study, but is most pronounced at the age of 8-9.

So, the results of the research indicate that mentally retarded students at the age of 8-11 retarded students aged 8-11 have poor right-hand and left-hand orientation, and even worse for the other person standing in front of them, and have difficulty correctly and completely enumerating objects on their right or left-hand side. Although these indicators improve somewhat with age, they are still low by the fourth grade.

Mentally retarded children have been found to be significantly inferior to their normally developing peers. This generally indicates that they have insufficient understanding of spatial relationships of objects and poor orientation.

In view of the above, it is most important to study the features of development of practical orientation of mentally retarded schoolchildren, connected, first of all, with domestic, educational, labour and other types of activity. In this connection, the subjects were offered the following practical tasks:

- how the right and left drawers of the table;

- o put a key in the right (left) pocket of a jacket;

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

165

© ®

All rights reserved

- determine whether a pencil was on the right or left side of a notebook lying on the table.

These tasks, in comparison with those presented earlier, are of a more abstract nature, they are less equal in degree of habituality for children and are aimed at determining the abilities and skills of mentally retarded children to correctly orient to the sides of the study table, to the location of this or that object in relation to others, etc.

The results of the experimental study of task performance show that interesting data have been obtained which will serve as a basis for the development of necessary means, methods and conditions for the development of elementary practical orientation in mentally retarded elementary school children.

The analysis of the results of the study allows us to highlight the following important points:

- entally retarded younger pupils lag behind their healthy peers in all the studied indicators;

- 9 year old have less developed practical spatial orientation skills than their older counterparts.

The listed errors are generally reduced to the use by mentally retarded pupils of imprecise, vague spatial characteristics instead of more differentiated ones. This vividly illustrates the limited and incomplete verbalization of spatial relations of objects in mentally retarded children.

Conclusions

We have identified disorders of spatial orientation and constructive praxis in normally developing and mentally retarded children aged 811, which clearly illustrate the limited and incomplete verbalization of spatial object relations in mentally retarded children. The listed errors as a whole are reduced to the use by mentally retarded schoolchildren of imprecise, vague spatial characteristics instead of more differentiated ones.

Thus, the results of the conducted experimental research testify to insufficient development of elementary practical orientation and understanding of spatial relations of objects at mentally retarded children of primary school age.

REFERENCES

1. Boguslavskaya V. F., Miroshnichenko A. V., Osipova A. A. The problem of meaning barriers in learning. The World of Science. Pedagogy and Psychology, 2019, no. 6, pp. 100. (In Russian) URL: https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=42596572

2. Marzi C. A. Asymmetry of interhemispheric communication. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2010, vol. 1 (3). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.53 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.53

3. Hugdahl K. Hemispheric asymmetry: Contributions from brain imaging. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2010, vol. 2 (5). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.122 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.122

4. Kalavathi P., Surya Prasath V. B. Automatic segmentation of cerebral hemispheres in MR human head scans. International Journal of Imaging Systems and Technology, 2016, vol. 26 (1). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ima.22152 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/ima.22152

5. Corballis M. C., Badzakova-Trajkov G., Haberling I. S. Right hand, left brain: Genetic and evolutionary bases of cerebral asymmetries for language and manual action. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2011, vol. 3 (1). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.158 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.158_

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today

166

© ®

All rights reserved

6. Dale Stevens W., Nathan Spreng R. Resting-state functional connectivity MRI reveals active processes central to cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2014, vol. 5 (2). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs. 1275 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs. 1275

7. Migliorelli C., Medina-Rivera I., Bachiller A., Tost A., Alonso J. F., López-Sala A., Armstrong J., O'Callahan M. d. M., Pineda M., Mañanas M. A., Romero S., García-Cazorla Á. Cognitive stimulation has potential for brain activation in individuals with Rett syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2021, vol. 66 (3). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jir.12902 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/jir. 12902

8. Lisova N. A., Shilov S. N. The role of activation processes cerebral cortex in formation of stress resistance in students with different temperamental characteristics. Siberian Bulletin of Special Education, 2015, no. 2, pp. 52-57. (In Russian) URL: https://elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=23651432

9. Corballis M. C., Forster B. Interhemispheric transfer of colour and shape information in the presence and absence of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia, 2000, vol. 38 (1), pp. 32-45. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00050-0

10. Reuter-Lorenz P. A., Jonides J., Smith E. E., Hartley A., Miller A., Marshuetz C., Koeppe R. A. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2000, vol. 12, pp. 174-187. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561814

11. Azatyan T. Y. Sensory Asymmetry assessement of 8-11 years old children with and without learning disabilities. Armenian Journal of Special Education Scientific Methodological Journal, 2021, vol. 3 (1), pp. 98-104. DOI: https://doi.org/10.24234/se.2021.3.1

12. Azatyan T. Y. Brain interhemispheric interaction in children with mental disabilities with spatial orientation disorders. Armenian Journal of Special Education Scientific Methodological Journal, 2022, vol. 5 (1), pp. 103-113. DOI: https://doi.org/10.24234/se.v5i1

13. Khokhlov N. A., Kovyazina M. S. Lateral signs, structural-level characteristics of intelligence, and mathematical abilities. Asymmetry, 2013, vol. 7 (3), pp. 32-52. (In Russian) URL: https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=20860062

14. Dunst C. J., Trivette C. M., Hamby D. W. Meta-analysis of family-centered helpgiving practices research. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 2007, vol. 13 (4), pp. 370-378. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20176

15. Wehmeyer M., Palmer S. Adult outcomes for students with cognitive disabilities three-years after high school: The impact of self-determination. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2003, vol. 38 (2), pp. 131-144. URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879591

16. Wehmeyer M. L., Palmer S. B. Adult outcomes for students with cognitive disabilities three-years after high school: The impact of self-determination. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2003, vol. 38 (2), pp. 131-144. URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879591

17. Pankova N. B., Alchinova I. B., Kovaleva O. I., Lebedeva M. A., Khlebnikova N. N., Cherepov A. B., Noskin L. A., Karganov M. Y. Correlation between the accuracy and speed of hand control in primary schoolchildren and the amount of screen time. Science for Education Today, 2021, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 142-160. (In Russian) DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15293/2658-6762.2103.08

18. Ahsan M. T., Deppeler J. M., Sharma U. Predicting pre-service teachers' preparedness for inclusive education: Bangladeshi pre-service teachers' attitudes and perceived teaching-efficacy for inclusive education. Cambridge Journal of Education, 2013, vol. 43 (4), pp. 517-535. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/0305764X.2013.834036

Science for Education Today

2023, vol. 13, issue 1

http://en.sciforedu.ru

ISSN 2658-6762

19. Paju B., Raty L., Pirttimaa R., Kontu E. The school staffs perception of their ability to teach special educational needs pupils in inclusive settings in Finland. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 2016, vol. 20 (8), pp. 801-815. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13603116.2015.1074731

20. Hunt P., Soto G., Maier J., Doering K. Collaborative teaming to support students at risk and students with severe disabilities in general education classroom. Exceptional Children, 2003, vol. 69, pp. 315332. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290306900304

21. Xiang L., Crow T., Roberts N. Automatic analysis of cross-sectional cerebral asymmetry on 3D in vivo MRI scans of human and chimpanzee. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 2019, vol. 97 (6). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jnr.24391 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24391

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

22. Helland T., Morken F., Bless J. J., Valderhaug H. V., Eiken M., Helland W. A., Torkildsen J. v. K. Auditive training effects from a dichotic listening app in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 2018, vol. 24 (4). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/dys.1600 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1600

23. Kovyazina M. S. Methods of objective study of mental processes in clinical psychology. Questions of Psychology, 2013, no. 1, pp. 66-71. (In Russian) URL: https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=21195530

24. Horn E., Lieber J., Sandall S., Schwartz I., Worley R. Supporting young children's IEP goals in inclusive settings through embedded learning opportunities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 2002, vol. 20, pp. 208-223. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/027112140002000402

25. Camilo M. G. The Elkonin-Davidov system: a grove of developmental teaching theory. Revista Científica Multidisciplinar Núcleo do Conhecimento, 2021, vol. 6, pp. 142-158. DOI: https://doi.org/10.32749/nucleodoconhecimento .com .br/education/developmental-teaching

Submitted: 28 October 2022 Accepted: 10 January 2023 Published: 28 February 2023

Tereza Yurevna Azatyan

Contribution of the co-author: collection of materials and initiation of research; definition of research methodology; analysis of research data. Olga Yuryevna Piskun

Contribution of the co-author: formulation of a scientific problem research; structuring and literature review.

G 0

This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. (CC BY 4.0).

The authors' stated contribution:

Information about competitive interests:

The authors claim that they do not have competitive interests.

Information about the Authors

Tereza Yurevna Azatyan

Candidate of Pedagogical Sciences, Associate Professor,

Armenian State Institute of Physical Culture and Sport;

Associate Professor, Head,

Department of Special Pedagogy and Psychology,

Armenian State Pedagogical University after Khachatur Abovyan,

Tigran Mets ave. Yerevan, Armenia.

ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7634-7727

E-mail: atereza222@gmail.com (Corresponding Author)

Olga Yuryevna Piskun

Candidate of Pedagogical Sciences, Associate Professor, Department of Correctional Pedagogy and Psychology, Head,

Resource Center for Support of Students with Disabilities, Novosibirsk State Pedagogical University, 630126, 28 Vilyuiskaya Str., Novosibirsk, Russian Federation. ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4953-6733 E-mail: o-piskun@yandex.ru

2023. Том 13. № 1

http://sciforedu.ru

ISSN 2658-6762

УДК 612.7+376+371 Научная статья / Research Full Article

DOI: 10.15293/2658-6762.2301.08 Язык статьи: английский / Article language: English

Исследование нарушений пространственной ориентации и конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет

Т. Ю. Азатян1, О. Ю. Пискун2

1 Армянский государственный институт физической культуры и спорта, Ереван, Армения 2 Новосибирский государственный педагогический университет, Новосибирск, Россия

Проблема и цель. Способность ориентироваться в пространстве является одним из основных условий гармоничного развития человека.

Проблема ориентации человека в пространстве достаточно многогранна. Она включает в себя как представления о величине и форме предметов, так и умение различать расположение объектов в пространстве, понимание различных пространственных отношений. Понятие пространственной ориентации включает в себя оценку расстояний, размеров, формы, взаимного расположения объектов и их положения относительно ориентирующегося.

У умственно отсталых детей пространственная ориентировка существенно затруднена, так как нарушение психического развития не только ограничивает их двигательную активность, но и отрицательно влияет на развитие временных представлений, функции опорно-двигательного аппарата и приводит к снижению компенсаторных процессов.

В связи с вышеизложенным, мы поставили цель выявить и научно обосновать нарушения пространственной ориентации и конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет.

Методология.

Методологической основой исследования послужили научные работы ряда авторов, посвященные изучению нервных функций у детей с нарушениями психического развития и межпо-лушарной асимметрии мозга, регуляции этих функций, выявлению трудностей, моделированию системы работы с этими детьми и нарушений конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет (Богуславская, Мирошниченко, 2019, Марю-тина, Ермолаев 2001, Ройтер-Лоренц и другие, 2000, Азатян, 2022).

Библиографическая ссылка: Азатян Т. Ю., Пискун О. Ю. Исследование нарушений пространственной ориентации и конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет // Science for Education Today. - 2023. - Т. 13, № 1. - С. 159-174. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15293/2658-6762.2204.08

Aemop для корреспонденции: Тереза Юрьевна Азатян, atereza222(ci>gmail.com © Т. Ю. Азатян, О. Ю. Пискун, 2023

Science for Education Today

2023. Том 13. № 1 http://sciforedu.ru/ ISSN 2658-6762

Исследование проводилось в научно-исследовательской лаборатории факультета специального и инклюзивного образования Армянского государственного педагогического университета им. Абовяна и научно-исследовательской лаборатории Армянского государственного института физической культуры и спорта.

В исследовании принял участие 131 ребенок в возрасте от 8 до 11 лет, из них 73 здоровых школьника и 58 детей с легкой умственной отсталостью.

Степень умственной отсталости оценивалась на основании медицинских показаний, степени социальной адаптации, уровня интеллектуального функционирования и освоения школьной программы в процессе обучения в специальном учебном заведении.

Результаты. В статье рассматриваются основные вопросы исследования пространственной ориентации и конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет, дается научное обоснование проблемы с учетом ранних исследований, однако проблема умственно отсталых и нормально развивающихся детей данной возрастной категории остается практически неизученной.

Анализ результатов исследования позволяет авторам выделить и обобщить следующие важные моменты:

- умственно отсталые дети начальной школы отстают от своих здоровых сверстников по всем изучаемым показателям;

- у 8- и 9-летних детей практические навыки пространственной ориентации развиты хуже, чем у их старших сверстников;

- у умственно отсталых сверстников недостаточно развиты навыки пространственной ориентировки и конструктивный праксис, что затрудняет понимание пространственных отношений между объектами.

По результатам экспериментального исследования выполнения заданий получены важные данные, которые послужат основой для разработки необходимых средств, методов и условий для развития элементарной практической ориентировки у умственно отсталых младших школьников.

Нами выявлены нарушения пространственной ориентировки и конструктивного праксиса у нормально развивающихся и умственно отсталых детей 8-11 лет, которые наглядно иллюстрируют ограниченность и неполноту вербализации пространственных отношений объектов у умственно отсталых детей. Перечисленные ошибки в целом сводятся к использованию умственно отсталыми школьниками неточных, расплывчатых пространственных характеристик вместо более дифференцированных.

Заключение. Таким образом, результаты проведенного экспериментального исследования свидетельствуют о недостаточном развитии элементарной практической ориентировки и понимания пространственных отношений объектов у умственно отсталых детей младшего школьного возраста.

Ключевые слова: межполушарное взаимодействие; функции мозга; нарушения пространственной ориентации; умственная отсталость; конструктивный праксис; психическое развитие детей; начальная школа.

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today Все права защищены

2023. Том 13. № 1 http://sciforedu.ru/

ISSN 2658-6762

СПИСОК ЛИТЕРАТУРЫ

1. Богуславская В. Ф., Мирошниченко А. В., Осипова А. А. Проблема смысловых барьеров в обучении // Мир науки. Педагогика и психология. - 2019. - № 6. - C. 100. URL: https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=42596572

2. Marzi C. A. Asymmetry of interhemispheric communication // Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. - 2010. - Vol. 1 (3). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.53 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.53

3. Hugdahl K. Hemispheric asymmetry: contributions from brain imaging // Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. - 2010. - Vol. 2 (5). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.122 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.122

4. Kalavathi P., Surya Prasath V. B. Automatic segmentation of cerebral hemispheres in MR human head scans // International Journal of Imaging Systems and Technology. - 2016. - Vol. 26 (1). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ima.22152 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/ima.22152

5. Corballis M. C., Badzakova-Trajkov G., Hàberling I. S. Right hand, left brain: genetic and evolutionary bases of cerebral asymmetries for language and manual action // Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. - 2011. - Vol. 3 (1). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.158 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.158

6. Dale Stevens W., Nathan Spreng R. Resting-state functional connectivity MRI reveals active processes central to cognition // Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. - 2014. -Vol. 5 (2). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1275 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1275

7. Migliorelli C., Medina-Rivera I., Bachiller A., Tost A., Alonso J. F., López-Sala A., Armstrong J., O'Callahan M. d. M., Pineda M., Mañanas M. A., Romero S., García-Cazorla Á. Cognitive stimulation has potential for brain activation in individuals with Rett syndrome // Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. - 2021. - Vol. 66 (3). URL: https: //onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jir.12902 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12902

8. Лисова Н. А., Шилов С. Н. Роль процессов активации коры головного мозга в формировании стрессоустойчивости у студенток с различными темпераментными характеристиками // Сибирский вестник специального образования. - 2015. - № 2. - С. 52-57. URL: https://elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=23651432

9. Corballis M. C., Forster B. Interhemispheric transfer of colour and shape information in the presence and absence of the corpus callosum // Neuropsychologia. - 2000. - Vol. 38 (1). - P. 3245. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00050-0

10. Reuter-Lorenz P. A., Jonides J., Smith E. E., Hartley A., Miller A., Marshuetz C., Koeppe R. A. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET // Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. - 2000. - Vol. 12. - P. 174-187. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561814

11. Azatyan T. Y. Sensory Asymmetry Assessement of 8-11 years old Children with and without Learning Disabilities // Armenian Journal of Special Education Scientific Methodological Journal. - 2021. - Vol. 3 (1). - P. 98-104. DOI: https://doi.org/10.24234/se.2021.3.1

12. Azatyan T. Y. Brain Interhemispheric Interaction in Children with Mental Disabilities with Spatial Orientation Disorders // Armenian Journal of Special Education Scientific Methodological Journal. - 2022. - Vol. 5 (1). - P. 103-113. DOI: https://doi.org/10.24234/se.v5i1

13. Хохлов Н. А., Ковязина М. С. Латеральные признаки, структурно-уровневые характеристики интеллекта и математические способности // Асимметрия. - 2013. - Т. 7, № 3. - С. 32-52. URL: https://www.elibrary.ru/item.asp?id=20860062

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today Все права защищены

172

2023. Том 13. № 1 http://sciforedu.ru/ ISSN 2658-6762

14. Dunst C. J., Trivette C. M., Hamby D. W. Meta-analysis of family-centered helpgiving practices research // Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. - 2007. -Vol. 13 (4). - P. 370-378. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20176

15. Wehmeyer M., Palmer S. Adult outcomes for students with cognitive disabilities three-years after high school: The impact of self-determination // Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities. - 2003. - Vol. 38 (2). - P. 131-144. URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879591

16. Wehmeyer M. L., Palmer S. B. Adult outcomes for students with cognitive disabilities three-years after high school: The impact of self-determination // Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities. - 2003. - Vol. 38 (2). - P. 131-144. URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879591

17. Панкова Н. Б., Алчинова И. Б., Ковалёва О. И., Лебедева М. А., Хлебникова Н. Н., Черепов А. Б., Носкин Л. А., Карганов М. Ю. Зависимость точностных и скоростных показателей психомоторной координации при работе руками у младших школьников от уровня компьютерной нагрузки // Science for Education Today. - 2021. - № 3. - С. 142-160. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15293/2658-6762.2103.08

18. Ahsan M. T., Deppeler J. M., Sharma U. Predicting Pre-Service Teachers' Preparedness for Inclusive Education: Bangladeshi Pre-Service Teachers' Attitudes and Perceived Teaching-Efficacy for Inclusive Education // Cambridge Journal of Education. - 2013. - Vol. 43 (4). -P. 517-535. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/0305764X.2013.834036

19. Paju B., Raty L., Pirttimaa R., Kontu E. The School Staffs Perception of Their Ability to Teach Special Educational Needs Pupils in Inclusive Settings in Finland // International Journal of Inclusive Education. - 2016. - Vol. 20 (8). - P. 801-815. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13603116.2015.1074731

20. Hunt P., Soto G., Maier J., Doering K. Collaborative teaming to support students at risk and students with severe disabilities in general education classroom // Exceptional Children. - 2003. - vol. 69. -pp. 315-332. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/001440290306900304

21. Xiang L., Crow T., Roberts N. Automatic analysis of cross-sectional cerebral asymmetry on 3D in vivo MRI scans of human and chimpanzee // Journal of Neuroscience Research. - 2019. -Vol. 97 (6). URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jnr.24391 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24391

22. Helland T., Morken F., Bless J. J., Valderhaug H. V., Eiken M., Helland W. A., Torkildsen J. v. K. Auditive training effects from a dichotic listening app in children with dyslexia // Dyslexia. -2018. - Vol. 24 (4). URL:https://onlinelibrary .wiley.com/doi/ 10.1002/dys.1600 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1600

23. Ковязина М. С. Объективные методики исследования психических процессов в клинической психологии // Вопросы психологии. - 2013. - № 1. - С. 66-71. URL: https://www. elibrary. ru/item. asp ?id=21195530

24. Horn E., Lieber J., Sandall S., Schwartz I., Worley R. Supporting young children's IEP goals in inclusive settings through embedded learning opportunities // Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. - 2002. - Vol. 20. - P. 208-223. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/027112140002000402

25. Camilo M. G. The Elkonin-Davidov system: a grove of developmental teaching theory // Revista Científica Multidisciplinar Núcleo do Conhecimento. - 2021. - Vol. 06. - P. 142-158. DOI: https://doi.org/10.32749/nucleodoconhecimento.com.br/education/developmental-teaching

Поступила: 28 октября 2022 Принята: 10 января 2023 Опубликована: 28 февраля 2023

Science for Education Today

2023. Том 13. № 1 http://sciforedu.ru/ ISSN 2658-6762

Заявленный вклад авторов:

Азатян Тереза Юрьевна: сбор материалов и инициация исследования; определение методологии исследования; анализ данных исследования. Пискун Ольга Юрьевна: постановка научной проблемы исследования; структурирование и обзор литературы.

Информация о конфликте интересов:

Авторы заявляют об отсутствии конфликта интересов.

Информация об авторах

Азатян Тереза Юрьевна

кандидат педагогических наук, доцент,

Армянский государственный институт физической культуры и спорта, Ереван, Армения; доцент, заведующий,

кафедра специальной педагогики и психологии,

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Армянский государственный педагогический университет им. Х. Абовяна, Проспект Тиграна Меца, 17, Ереван, Армения. ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3218-257X E-mail: atereza222@gmail.com

Пискун Ольга Юрьевна

кандидат педагогических наук, доцент,

кафедра коррекционной педагогики и психологии,

руководитель,

ресурсный центр сопровождения обучающихся с ограниченными возможностями здоровья,

Новосибирский государственный педагогический университет, Вилюйская ул., 28, Новосибирск, 630126, Россия. ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4953-6733 E-mail: o-piskun@yandex.ru

© 2011-2023 Science for Education Today Все права защищены

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.