Научная статья на тему 'Social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of Tuvan throat singing'

Social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of Tuvan throat singing Текст научной статьи по специальности «Искусствоведение»

CC BY
429
79
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Журнал
Новые исследования Тувы
Scopus
ВАК
ESCI
Область наук
Ключевые слова
TUVA / TUVANS / MUSIC / THROAT SINGING / FUNCTION OF MUSIC / ТУВА / ТУВИНЦЫ / МУЗЫКА / ГОРЛОВОЕ ПЕНИЕ / ФУНКЦИИ МУЗЫКИ

Аннотация научной статьи по искусствоведению, автор научной работы — Hsu Shen-Mou

Music is pervasive across human cultures and throughout times. Particularly, music is of great importance socially. Like many cultures’ use of music, throat singing or xöömei, the most distinguished aspect of Tuva’s music, contributes significantly to social communication, emotional expression, social bonding and religious rituals. Acknowledgment and consideration of current social cognitive findings of music may thus provide a better insight into the nature of throat singing. To date, evidence has indicated that, similar to language, music is a fundamental channel of communication, and these two constructs may have common origins in a single communicative system. Moreover, music may modulate neural activity in the brain structures associated with emotions and alter our autonomic responses. In addition to information sharing, music thus has the capacity to convey emotions. This ability may further render music a powerful mechanism to facilitate social bonding and ritual practice, as individuals’ internal states during these social events become synchronized through musical engagement. In conclusion, I suggest that those social cognitive perspectives may point toward new directions for a continuing discourse on our understanding of throat singing.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «Social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of Tuvan throat singing»

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017 Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

DOI: 10.25178/nit.2017.2.8

social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of tuvan throat singing

Shen-Mou Hsu

National Taiwan University, Taiwan

социальные и познавательные функции музыки на примере горлового пения тувинцев

Шень-Моу Сюй

Национальный Университет Тайваня,

Тайвань

Music is pervasive across human cultures and throughout times. Particularly, music is of great importance socially. Like many cultures' use of music, throat singing or xoomei, the most distinguished aspect of Tuva's music, contributes significantly to social communication, emotional expression,

social bonding and religious rituals. Acknowledgment and consideration of current social cognitive findings of music may thus provide a better insight into the nature of throat singing. To date, evidence has indicated that, similar to language, music is a fundamental channel of communication, and these two constructs may have common origins in a single communicative system. Moreover, music may modulate neural activity in the brain structures associated with emotions and alter our autonomic responses. In addition to information sharing, music thus has the capacity to convey emotions. This ability may further render music a powerful mechanism to facilitate social bonding and ritual practice, as individuals' internal states during these social events become synchronized

Музыка является неотъемлемой частью практически всех человеческих культур во все времена. Музыка выполняет множество социальных и познавательных функций. Горловое пение (хоомей) — наиболее известный вид музыкального искусства тувинцев — занимает важное место в общественной жизни: оно выражает общественные связи, показывает эмоции исполнителей, сопровождает социальное общение и религиозные ритуалы. Признание и постижение общественных и познавательных аспектов музыки могут помочь лучше понять природу горлового пения. До настоящего времени исследования указывали, что, подобно языку, музыка является одним из основных каналов коммуникации. Более того, музыка может модулировать нервную деятельность в мозговых структурах, связанных с чувствами и видоизменять наши автономные реакции. Таким образом, в дополнение к функции обмена информацией, музыка способна передавать чувства. Это помогает музыке выступать мощным механизмом для установления общественных связей и выражения ритуальной практики, поскольку способствует синхронизации внутреннего состояния индивидуумов во время тех или иных общественных действий.

Shen-MouHsu, Researcher, Imaging Center for Integrated Body, Mind and Culture Research, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan (R.O.C.). Postal address: 49 Fanglan Rd., Da'an Dist, Taipei, 10617, Taiwan (R.O.C). Email: smhsu@ntu.edu.tw Шень-Моу Сюй — научный сотрудник Центра интегративных исследований тела, разума и культуры Национального Университета Тайваня, г. Тайбэй. Адрес: 49 Fanglan Rd., Da'an Dist, Taipei, 10617, Taiwan (R.O.C). Email: smhsu@ntu.edu.tw

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017 Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

through musical engagement.

In conclusion, I suggest that those social cognitive perspectives may point toward new directions for a continuing discourse on our understanding of throat singing.

Keywords: Tuva; Tuvans; music; throat singing; function of music

В заключение автор напоминает, что упомянутые общественные познавательные функции горлового пения могут стать новым направлением для исследований в области тувинской музыки.

Ключевые слова: Тува; тувинцы; музыка; горловое пение; функции музыки

Introduction

Music is pervasive across human cultures and throughout times. Appreciation for it appears to be a predisposition. Infants as young as four months already tend to favor consonant music rather than a dissonant one (Zentner, Kagan, 1996).

Equally intriguing is the finding that infants are better at detecting changes in diatonic melodies which conform to keys of major or minor scales, rather than melodies that violate these conventions (Trehub, Thorpe, Trainor, 1990). However, our drive to engage in music and with its reasons for existence has not been readily explained.

On one hand, music may represent an evolutionary adaptation with biological significance, such as being a sexually selected trait that aids courtship in a manner similar to bird singing (Darwin, 2007). On the other hand, music may be merely an invention of humans (Patel, 2010) or a byproduct that evolves to support language (Pinker, 1999).

Despite the ongoing debate surrounding the evolutionary status of music, it is widely conceived that music serves important social functions. For example, music involves communication, notably during parent-child singing of lullabies (Trehub, 2003). Moreover, music has capacity to convey emotion and is an important potential source of pleasure (Blood, Zatorre, 2001; Menon, Levitin, 2005).

Music may also bind groups of people together to increase solidarity and thereby help early humans to thrive (Roederer, 1984). Last but not the least, music is widely observed and constitutes an abstract representation in human communal and religious rituals (Alcorta, Sosis, 2005; Becker, 2001).

Throat singing or xoomei, the most distinguished aspect of Tuva's music, revolves around the principle that a vocalist produces multiple notes at one time. The lower note is the fundamental tone of the voice and sounds like a sustained drone. The second corresponds to one of the harmonic partials. This unique singing style makes no exception in terms of their roles in social functions, particularly given that throat singing has a rich bond to Tuvan culture and traditions (Levin, Edgerton, 1999).

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

In this mini review, I focus on discussing four interrelated social functions of music: communication, emotion, social bonding and ritual, and their potential relevance to the nature of throat singing. Hopefully, these social cognitive perspectives may contribute to a continuing discourse on our understanding of throat singing.

Music as a channel of communication

The Tuvan society has strong traditions of animism, the belief that natural objects have souls or are inhabited by spirits of rivers, mountains, and many other aspects of the world (Levin, Suzukei, 2006). Because Tuvan people believe that sound is a channel preferred by the spirits of nature to reveal themselves and to communicate with the other living beings, throat singing serves as specific language that Tuvan shamans developed to communicate with those spirits.

In addition to the spiritual context, throat singing is also performed in daily life from lulling a baby to sleep to calling animals from a distance (Pegg, 1992). Researchers even suggest that lullabies might be a formative force that contributes to the development of throat singing (Van Tongeren, 2002), which strengthens the idea that throat singing plays an essential role in social communication in Tuvan society.

Music is indeed a fundamental channel of communication (Kraus, Slater, 2016) and bears resemblance to language (Brown, 2000; Patel, 2003; Tillmann, 2012). Both music and language are organized according to given rules which combine basic components into higher-order structures. Moreover, decoding both constructs requires a fine-grained analysis of acoustic information which unfolds simultaneously over multiple timescales.

S. Brown proposes that music and language may have common origins in a single communicative system, then become separate capacities through a process of divergence and functional specialization (Brown, 2000). Such an intimate connection between music and language has been further corroborated by a growing body of cognitive neuroscientific evidence, indicating that overlapping and shared neural resources are recruited for musical and linguistic processing (Koelsch, 2011; Patel, 2012).

Moreover, music which emanates from language could further accentuate linguistic expression. This would account for the fact that songs generally contain words which often function as a kind of enhancement and stylization of the ideas being expressed in the text. Ancient Chinese literature, the Book of Documents or "Shanshu", also expresses the idea that "Singing prolongs the sounds of speech; The sounds of singing follow prolonged speech sounds; Musical pitches temper the sounds of singing". As can be similarly found in throat singing, xoomei phrases are often interspersed within Tuvan songs with topical words.

During the communication processing of sounds in music, our brains have to determine the physicality from the ears and interpret the underlying information

179

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

patterns that might inform us. However, our natural environments are complex, and there may be different inputs that integrate auditory information. Previous research has shown that visual information may affect the brain's ability to construct a percept of sound. As demonstrated by the McGurk effect (McGurk, MacDonald, 1976), when a syllable, "ba" for instance, is repeatedly dubbed on to a lip movement for "ga", we would perceive a hybrid percept, the syllable "da".

Similarly for the sounds as complex as music, evidence has shown that the perception of music performances can be significantly influenced by a performer's movements (Tsay, 2013). Intriguingly, throat singing is recognized as the quintessential achievement of Tuvans' mimesis based on perceptual cues from their surrounding environment. Legend says that Tuvans developed the art of throat singing by imitating the sounds of nature and the calls and cries of animals rich in harmonics (Levin, Edgerton, 1999).

Drawing on the aforementioned findings, could it be possible that natural inputs other than sounds have any influence during the development of throat singing? It would be intriguing to probe to what extent the ambient cues derived from Tuvans' inhabitants interact with the linguistic system and ultimately determine throat singing.

Music as communication of emotion

For Tuvan herders, throat singing also serves as a way to express feelings of exultation. As quoted in van Tongeren, anonymous singer once described:

"Imagine you're a herdsman and you sit on your horse and you're going somewhere, far away. The moment that you're bored or when you are enchanted by something, like the steppes or the taiga, you immediately have to sing what you feel. If you sit on your horse and you sing, then it hears that you are singing. And it will go even faster, because it likes your songs" (Van Tongeren, 2002: 56).

From the social cognitive perspective, music is not only communicative in the sense of sharing information, but also concerned with transmission of emotions (Panksepp, Trevarthen, 2008; Roederer, 1984). Emotion often accompanies our musical experiences and emotional impact is one of the main reasons that people listen to music (Sloboda, O'Neill, 2001).

Cognitive neuroscientific studies to date have demonstrated that music may modulate neural activity in the brain structures associated with emotions (Koelsch, 2010). Particularly, during listening to pleasure music, dopaminergic neural systems which are associated with reward (a construct involving in craving or wanting states) can be activated (Blood, Zatorre, 2001; Menon, Levitin, 2005). In addition, there is a close connection between music and our physiological responses (Gomez, Danuser, 2007; Krumhansl, 2002). For example, fast and staccato music may evoke faster breathing, higher minute ventilation, skin conductance, and heart rate. In principle, 180

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

tempo, accentuation, and rhythmic articulation are the features that most strongly correlate with physiological responses.

Although it is unclear whether different styles and techniques of throat singing are correlated with different kinds of emotions, the link between musical structures and emotions has been previously reported (Bresin, Friberg, 2011; Scherer, 1995). Evidence suggests that sadness may be conveyed by slow tempos, low intensity and legato articulation, whereas happiness is conveyed by fast tempo, high intensity and staccato articulation. Gomez and Danuser's study (Gomez, Danuser, 2007) further reveals that mode, harmonic complexity, and rhythmic articulation can better characterize whether emotional experience is negative or positive, whereas tempo, accentuation, and rhythmic articulation can better discriminate whether emotion is in high or low intensity. Therefore, internal structure of the music plays an essential role in the induction of specific emotions and this principle may be applicable to throat singing.

Music as a means of social bonding

Throat singing should not only be construed as a form of communication, but also be considered in terms of how the music style is embedded in social contexts. For example, throat singing is frequently performed at weddings or a celebration at a nobleman's in Mongolia (Pegg, 1992). Similarly in most cultures, music is employed in consequential events such as seasonal festivals. Through music engagement, individuals' emotional states in these social events are shared and become synchronized, thereby generating heightened pleasure and decreasing conflicts, and the feelings of social cohesion is ultimately fostered (Benzon, 2001; Mithen, 2005; Roederer, 1984).

In early eras, social bonding could arise from community dancing to music, manifested by the coordinated movements of military drills (McNeill, 1995). Such coordinated drills resulted in camaraderie via physical or emotional synchrony, a phenomenon which today can still be witnessed at football games or rock concerts. One recent study provides supporting evidence that when rhythmic activities like music are performed by groups of people, they tend to become synchronized, which reflects social coordination (Kirschner, Tomasello, 2010).

In this empirical study, two groups of 4-year-old children either interacted with one another in the context of traditional music that included dancing, singing and playing or interacted with one another without music. The results show that subsequently, children in the music group increased spontaneous cooperative and helpful behavior relative to the control group. Thus, joint music making encourages individuals to keep collective intention and emotions to behave more cooperatively towards one another. To summarize, music, possibly throat singing as well, offers an opportunity

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

for group catharsis and thereby facilitates the formation or manifestation of group identity.

Music in religious contexts

In the past, throat singing seemed to be reserved for shamans and was an important preparation for the shamanic rituals (Van Tongeren, 2002). It is possible that throat singing stems from such ritual practice. In an equivalent line, in many cultures, music is used to enter trance, a base for shamanism and early religions (Richter, Ostovar, 2016). Generally speaking, music is inseparable from communal and religious ritual (Becker, 2001). Human rituals share basic structural components of pattern, sequence and repetition. These components are amplified and intensified via the incorporation of recurrent music (Alcorta, Sosis, 2005).

As described above, music has the capacity to induce emotional effects. In addition, our autonomic functions, including respiration, and cardiovascular function, are altered in response to eternal musical rhythm (Gomez, Danuser, 2007). Not surprisingly, emotion, especially happiness, is frequently evoked in religious ritual (Becker, 2001) and participants engaged in ritual trance demonstrate changes in autonomic functions (Alcorta, Sosis, 2005). Together, the capacity of music to entrain automatic states and emotions is instrumental in imbuing human rituals with emotional significance. This process may provide the basis for creating and synchronizing motivational states in ritual participants ( Alcorta, Sosis, 2005; Alcorta, Sosis, Finkel, 2008).

Conclusion

Humans are intensely social creatures and music is an important social endeavor. The pursuit of music may stem from our biological-based social nature, which continues to motivate musical activity from the past to the present. Music is a powerful means of human communication by which people can share meanings as well as emotions. This ability renders music a robust mechanism for creating cohesive groups and shaping human rituals. Like many cultures' use of music, throat singing is also prioritized around communication, social gathering, or shamanic rituals. Given the significant contribution of throat singing in the social domain of the Tuvan society, current social cognitive findings and approaches to music may stand as an excellent medium for better understanding the nature of throat singing.

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

Acknowledgments

The author thanks Tammy Ho for the comments on an early version of the manuscript.

Автор благодарит Тамми Хо за комментарии по первой версии рукописи ста-

Alcorta, C. S. and Sosis, R. (2005) Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols : The evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Human Nature, no. 16, pp. 323-359. DOI: 10.1007/s12110-005-1014-3

Alcorta, C. S., Sosis, R. and Finkel, D. (2008) Ritual harmony: toward an evolutionary theory of music. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, no. 31, pp. 576-577.

Becker, J. (2001) Anthropological Perspectives on Music and Emotion. In: Juslin P. N. and Sloboda J. A. (Eds.) Music and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pp. 135-160.

Benzon, W. (2001) Beethoven's anvil : music in mind and culture. New York: Basic Books.

Blood, A. J. and Zatorre, R. J. (2001) Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 98 (20). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.191355898

Bresin, R. and Friberg, A. (2011). Emotion rendering in music: range and characteristic values of seven musical variables. Cortex, no. 47(9), pp. 1068-1081.

Brown, S. (2000) The musilanguage model of music evolution. In: Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. and Brown S. (Eds.) The Origins of Music. Cambridge : MIT Press. Pp. 271-300.

Darwin, C. (2007) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (Concise ed.). New York: Plume.

Gomez, P. and Danuser, B. (2007) Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion, no. 7, pp. 377-387.

Kirschner, S. and Tomasello, M. (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution & Human Behavior, no. 31, pp. 354-364.

Koelsch, S. (2010). Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, no. 14, pp. 131-137.

Koelsch, S. (2011) Toward a neural basis of music perception — a review and updated model. Frontier in Psychology, no. 2, 110. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00110

Благодарности

REFERENCES

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

Kraus, N. and Slater, J. (2016). Beyond Words: How Humans Communicate Through Sound. Annual Review of Psychology, no. 67, pp. 83-103. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033318

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Krumhansl, C. L. (2002) Music: A link between cognition and emotion Current Directions in Psychological Science, no. 11, pp. 45-50.

Levin, T. C. and Edgerton, M. E. (1999). The throat singers of Tuva. Scientific American, no. 281, pp. 80-87.

Levin, T. C. and Suzukei, V. (2006). Where rivers and mountains sing: sound, music, and nomadism in Tuva and beyond. Bloomington : Indiana University Press.

McGurk, H. and MacDonald, J. (1976) Hearing lips and seeing voice. Nature, no. 264, pp. 307-340.

McNeill, W. H. (1995) Keeping together in time : dance and drill in human history. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. 196 p.

Menon, V. and Levitin, D. J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage, no. 28, pp. 175184. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053

Mithen, S. J. (2005) The singing Neanderthals : the origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

Panksepp, J. and Trevarthen, C. (2008). The neuroscience of emotion in music. In: Malloch S. and Trevarthen C. (Eds.) Communicative Musicality : Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pp. 105-146.

Patel, A. D. (2003) Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, no. 6, pp. 674-681. DOI: 10.1038/nn1082

Patel, A. D. (2010) Music, biological evolution, and the brain. In: Bailar M. (Ed.) Emerging Disciplines. Houston, TX: Houston UNiversity Press. Pp. 91-144.

Patel, A. D. (2012) Language, music, and the brain: a resource-sharing framework. In: Rebuschat P., Rohrmeier M., Hawkins J. and Cross I. (Eds.) Language and Music as Cognitive Systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pp. 204-223.

Pegg, C. (1992) Mongolian conceptualizations of overtone Singing. British Journal of Ethnomusicology, no. 1, pp. 31-54. DOI: 10.1080/09681229208567199

Pinker, S. (1999) How the mind works. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 882, pp. 119-127. DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08538.x

Richter, J. and Ostovar, R. (2016). «It Don't Mean a Thing if It Ain't Got that Swing» — an Alternative Concept for Understanding the Evolution of Dance and Music in Human Beings. Frontier in Human Neuroscience, 10, 485. DOI: 10.3389/ fnhum.2016.00485

www.nit.tuva.asia

№2

2017

Novye issledovaniia Tuvy

Roederer, J. G. (1984) The search for a survival value of music. Music Perception, no. 1, pp. 350-356. DOI: 10.2307/40285265

Scherer, K. R. (1995) Expression of emotion in voice and music. Journal of Voice, no. 9, pp. 235-248.

Sloboda, J. A. and O'Neill, S. A. (2001) Emotions in everyday listening to music. In: Juslin P. N. and Sloboda J. A. (Eds.) Music and Emotion: Theory and Research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pp. 415-429.

Tillmann, B. (2012) Music and language perception: expectations, structural integration, and cognitive sequencing. Topic in Cognitive Science, no. 4(4), pp. 568584. DOI: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01209.x

Trehub, S. E. (2003) The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, no. 6, pp. 669-673. DOI: 10.1038/nn1084

Trehub, S. E., Thorpe, L. A. and Trainor, L. J. (1990) Infant's perception of good and bad melodies. Psychomusicology, 9 (1), pp. 5-19.

Tsay, C. J. (2013) Sight over sound in the judgment of music performance. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, no. 110, pp. 14580-14585. DOI: 10.1073/ pnas.1221454110

Van Tongeren, M. C. (2002) Overtone singing: physics and metaphysics of harmonics in east and west. Amsterdam : Fusica. ISBN 90-807163-1-6

Zentner, M. R. and Kagan, J. (1996) Perception of music by infants. Nature, 383, 29. DOI:10.1038/383029a0

Для цитирования:

Hsu Shen-Mou. Social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of Tuvan throat singing [Электронный ресурс] // Новые исследования Тувы. 2017, № 2. URL: https://nit.tuva.asia/nit/article/view/719 (дата обращения: ...). DOI: 10.25178/ nit.2017.2.8

For citation:

Hsu Shen-Mou. Social and cognitive functions of music based on the example of Tuvan throat singing. The New Research of Tuva, 2017, no. 1 [on-line] Available at: https:// nit.tuva.asia/nit/article/view/719 (accessed: ...). DOI: 10.25178/nit.2017.2.8

Submission date: 02.05.2017. Дата получения: 02.05.2017 г.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.