Научная статья на тему 'Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives'

Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives Текст научной статьи по специальности «Языкознание и литературоведение»

CC BY
0
0
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Ключевые слова
film / TV interview / speech / gesture / discourse space / кино / телеинтервью / речь / жест / дискурсивное пространство

Аннотация научной статьи по языкознанию и литературоведению, автор научной работы — Киосе Мария Ивановна, Леонтьева Анна Васильевна, Агафонова Ольга Владимировна

The article explores two media discourse formats, film and TV interview as manifesting the specific distribution of two communicative modalities — speech and gesture — in the multimodal deixis of discourse space construal in narratives. To identify the attribution of speech and gesture to two media formats, the study contrasts the plot contents and viewer engagement as two major coordinates of discourse space in media. We hypothesize that since these two formats display differences in their plot content and viewer engagement characteristics, their distribution might affect the way the actors employ speech and gesture to present the discourse space. The research data are 20 film and TV interview narratives with 10 highly rated Russian actors (5 men and 5 women). The study exploits the method of multimodal analysis to determine the contingency patterns of discursive schemata presenting discourse space in speech and of discursive schemata presenting object of reference, move and frame of reference in deictic gesture. The results show that multimodal deixis contributes to intensifying both types of these characteristics. While multimodal deixis in film manifests the preference for metonymic pointing, placement indication and speech event construal; in TV interview, we observe the significant prevalence of metaphoric pointing, path indication and narrative event construal. Additionally, TV interviews display the speech schemata specifying information and accentuating the place of referent within the speech or narrative event. Overall, the study shows that there are two basic types of discourse characteristics which produce higher distinctions in multimodal deixis between film and TV interview formats; these are different narrative regimes and different combination of staged performance and spontaneity.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Полимодальный дейксис в медиадискурсе: нарративы кино и телеинтервью

В статье анализу подвергаются два жанра медиадискурса — кино и телеинтервью, — демонстрирующие специфический характер распределения коммуникативных модальностей речи и жеста в полимодальном дейксисе при конструировании дискурсивного пространства в нарративе. Для установления особенностей этого распределения в работе изучается «содержание сюжета» и «перспектива наблюдателя» как две основные координаты конструирования дискурсивного пространства в медиа. Гипотеза исследования заключается в том, что наличие различий в «содержании сюжета» и «перспективе наблюдателя» в рассматриваемых жанрах будет определять распределение речи и жеста в конструировании дискурсивного пространства актерами. Материалом анализа являются 20 фрагментов кино и телеинтервью с участием рейтинговых российских актеров (5 мужчин и 5 женщин). Основной метод исследования — полимодальный анализ, который применяется для установления паттернов сопряженности (совместной встречаемости) дискурсивных схем, использующихся для конструирования дискурсивного пространства в речи, и дискурсивных схем, служащих для конструирования объекта референции, движения и референциального фрейма в дейктических жестах. Результаты показали, что полимодальный дейксис демонстрирует специфические характеристики в кино и телеинтервью. Так, в кино превалирует метонимическое указание, указание на место при конструировании события речи; в то время как в телеинтервью чаще наблюдается метафорическое указание, указание на путь при конструировании события нарратива. Установлено, что в телеинтервью дискурсивные схемы в речи чаще представляют детализированную информацию, а также информацию, уточняющую расположение объекта референции в событии речи или в событии нарратива. В целом исследование позволило обнаружить два основных типа дискурсивных характеристик, определяющих различия в полимодальном дейксисе кино и телеинтервью; к ним отнесены разные нарративные формы и различное сочетание постановочного и спонтанного в поведении актеров.

Текст научной работы на тему «Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives»

UDC 81-114.2

MeflManMHrBMCTMKa. 2024. TOM 11, № 1

Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives*

M. I. Kiose, A. V. Leonteva, O. V. Agafonova

Moscow State Linguistic University, 38, ul. Ostozhenka, Moscow, 119034, Russian Federation Institute of Linguistics of the Russian Academy of Sciences, 1, Bolshoy Kislovsky per., Moscow, 125009, Russian Federation

For citation: Kiose M. I., Leonteva A. V., Agafonova O. V. (2024). Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives. Media Linguistics, 11 (1), 30-52. https://doi.org/10.21638/spbu22.2024.103

The article explores two media discourse formats, film and TV interview as manifesting the specific distribution of two communicative modalities — speech and gesture — in the multimodal deixis of discourse space construal in narratives. To identify the attribution of speech and gesture to two media formats, the study contrasts the plot contents and viewer engagement as two major coordinates of discourse space in media. We hypothesize that since these two formats display differences in their plot content and viewer engagement characteristics, their distribution might affect the way the actors employ speech and gesture to present the discourse space. The research data are 20 film and TV interview narratives with 10 highly rated Russian actors (5 men and 5 women). The study exploits the method of multimodal analysis to determine the contingency patterns of discursive schemata presenting discourse space in speech and of discursive schemata presenting object of reference, move and frame of reference in deictic gesture. The results show that multimodal deixis contributes to intensifying both types of these characteristics. While multimodal deixis in film manifests the preference for metonymic pointing, placement indication and speech event construal; in TV interview, we observe the significant prevalence of metaphoric pointing, path indication and narrative event construal. Additionally, TV interviews display the speech schemata specifying information and accentuating the place of referent within the speech or narrative event. Overall, the study shows that there are two basic types of discourse characteristics which produce higher distinctions in multimodal deixis between film and TV interview formats; these are different narrative regimes and different combination of staged performance and spontaneity.

Keywords: film, TV interview, speech, gesture, discourse space. Problem statement

In this study, we regard media as a community formed by a set of institutions, cultural practices, industries and ways in which people interact with the society [Masterman 1997; Couldry 2002] which appear in multimodal discourse formats with film and television being the most popular. Contrastive studies across multimodal media discourse formats commonly explore the differences in their semantic, pragmatic, cognitive, semiotic, func-

* This research was supported by the Ministry for Education in Russia, project no. 075-03-2020-013/3 "Multimodal analysis of communicative behavior in different types of spoken discourse" and was carried out at the Centre for Socio-Cognitive Discourse Studies at Moscow State Linguistic University.

© St. Petersburg State University, 2024

tional and social structure. These studies allow to model the discourse as a social event and to identify the contrastive discursive role of speech, image, sound as semiotic modalities [Dobrosklonskaya 2016; Djonov, Zhao 2013; Machin 2013]. Meanwhile, the input of communicative modalities like speech and spontaneous gesture into film and television formats of media discourse is often neglected, although their semiotic nature in cinematic discourse has been extensively discussed in literary criticism (see, for instance: [Eisenstein 1964-1968; Smith 2010; Bugaeva 2016]). Still, multimodal behavior may manifest specificity in different discourse formats and may even serve as a predictor of a media discourse format due to its staged performance character. In this study, we address two media discourse formats, film and TV interview, in the discourse genre of a narrative common for both formats, to contrast the multimodal behavior of a narrator as potentially dependent on the discourse format.

To proceed, the study develops the method which will allow to contrast the narrator's multimodal behavior in two media formats. The difficulty lies in the fact that whereas there is increasing research in multimodal behavior in single media discourse genres [Iriskh-anova 2019; Lavender 2021], the discursive strategies and discursive functions (with their discursive markers) which serve to explore the contingent gesture behavior in one discourse format may not appear in a different discourse format; therefore, they cannot be contrasted. Additionally, gestures are invariably multifunctional [Cienki, Mittelberg 2013; Iriskhanova, Cienki 2018] and consequently different gesture types can appear as contingent on the same discursive strategies and functions.

This accounts for the need to search for new methods of contrasting multimodal communicative behavior of discourse participants in film and television narratives. In the study, we address the multimodal behavior in media discourse formats as stimulated by the discourse categories which shape any discourse type, with the categories of TIME and SPACE being most influential [Kubryakova, Aleksandrova 1997] often integrally addressed as DISCOURSE SPACE [Plotnikova 2011] and explored in film [Pronin 2018]. Notably, DISCOURSE SPACE construal in film and television narratives manifests significant differences since each of the two "specific mediums create particular storytelling parameters, constraining some options while enabling others <.. .> regarding plot structures and viewer engagement" [Mittell 2007: 156]. In the multimodal narrative, these discourse categories are commonly represented via multimodal deixis, expressed in deictic gestures synchronized (used together) with speech markers relating to either time or space or both. Although multimodal deixis research has gained popularity in discourse studies [Kita, Ozyurek 2003; Pfeiffer 2010; Le Guen 2011; Mesh et al. 2021; Iriskhanova et al. 2022], it is still uncommon in contrastive analysis.

This paper proposes a multimodal method of revealing the discursive schemata of two media discourse formats, film and television featuring one discourse genre of narratives. Central to this paper is how the narrators' preference for DISCOURSE SPACE construal in a specific discourse format affects the choice of speech and gesture discursive schemata, or the patterns of discourse components construal. Our study is performed with highly ranked actors (five males and five females) who produce the narratives in two media formats, film and television interview. In these narratives, multiple examples of gesture use are documented, with deictic gestures being one of the four functional gesture types [Cienki, Mittelberg 2013] contingent on DISCOURSE SPACE construal in speech [Le Guen 2011]. Presumably, DISCOURSE SPACE construal manifested via multimodal

deixis in both speech and spontaneous gesture will serve to identify the differences in the multimodal behavior of actors performing narratives in two media discourse formats, film and television. We begin the paper by reviewing the theoretical and empirical background to research on two media formats as hypothetically manifesting different discursive schemata of DISCOURSE SPACE construal via multimodal deixis; and then introduce the method, design, and results of the contrastive study of the narrators' multimodal behavior in DISCOURSE SPACE construal in film and TV narratives.

Theoretical framework

Since the study contrasts multimodal deixis in speech and spontaneous gesture in the narratives in two media formats, film and TV interview, we address three research areas. First, we explore the media discourse formats of film and TV interview to reveal the specifics of DISCOURSE SPACE construal in narratives via their "plot structures and viewer engagement" [Mittell 2007: 156]. Next, we consider the multimodal deictic specificity of DISCOURSE SPACE construal in gestures and speech. Finally, we explore the coordination patterns of deictic gestures and speech suggesting that there may be a research algorithm for representing multimodal deixis via discursive schemata in the contrastive perspective.

DISCOURSE SPACE in film and television narratives

TIME and SPACE are generally considered as two major categories of discourse construal. While their relations in discourse may be complex with the category of SPACE representing physical, communicative, genre relations, and TIME representing objective and subjective conceptualizations, following E. Kubryakova and O. Aleksandrova we consider discourse as "a spatial representation of time filled with articulation of speech production or being filled with its creation" [Kubryakova, Aleksandrova 1997: 23]; therefore, the discourse category of TIME is conceptualized as integrated into a more complex category of SPACE, or rather DISCOURSE SPACE. Developing the idea of DISCOURSE SPACE, S. Plotnikova defines it as "a logically organized medium incorporating discourses and discourse participants or the people producing discourses. Logical organization of a medium presupposes its abstract extension which is the continuum involving time as one of its coordinates" [Plotnikova 2011: 154]. The idea of discourse as a DISCOURSE SPACE agrees with A. Kibrik's notion of discourse as "both a process developing in time and a structured object" [Kibrik 2009: 4]. DISCOURSE SPACE construal in film and television narratives manifests significant differences due to the fact that these media discourse formats represent a) different "plot structures" and b) different perspective or "viewer engagement" structures [Mittell 2007: 156].

Films as specific discourse formats have only recently become a focal point in discourse studies; however, they have been extendedly explored in narratology, aesthetic semiotics and literary criticism. These approaches commonly appeal to two semiotic components of films which are the actor's performance and the film director's performance. In narratol-ogy, films appear as manifesting various narrative stages, categories and signs [Chare, Wat-kins 2017], whereas aesthetic semiotics mostly addresses the poetics of cinema as a system of signs and means of aesthetically significant information [Deleuze 1986; Deleuze 1989;

Auerbach 2007]. In critical studies, the major focus is the discourse categories like time, space, perspective, emotivity, identity, etc. [Smith 2010; Bugaeva 2016]. In recent studies of film discourse the main focus is shifted towards the discursive strategies and discourse categories in single cinematic subdiscourses [Zykova 2021] or in discourse types (narrative, descriptive, expository, and argumentative [Longacre 1996]). Since we address DISCOURSE SPACE construal in film narratives, we specify its plot structure and viewer engagement structure which appear in this media discourse format. Considering the film narrative plot structure explored in both semiotic and discourse studies, we can specify its major characteristics: 1) varying significance of actor's performance (often termed as "gesture") and the film director's performance (termed as "image") in contributing to the narrative plot [Noys 2014; Harbord 2015; Agamben 2019]; 2) enhanced embodiment role appearing in accentuated gesturing and mimics [Kristeva 2014]; 3) the effects of cultural and aesthetic experience and insight (in both actor's and film director's performance) [Eisenstein 1964-1968, vol. 3] termed as "stadium" and "punctum" in R. Barthes study [Barthes 1981]; 4) sensory experience foregrounding [Mulvey 1988]; 5) presenting fictive events with an emphasis on emotionality, imagery, pragmaticity and logics [Ivanov 1976; Bugaeva 2016].

In terms of the viewer engagement structure characteristics, the studies name the following ones: 1) multimodal alignment of speech and exaggerated gesturing of the type "look into the camera" [Ciccognani 2018]; 2) viewer engagement gesture use which stimulates discourse dynamicity [Iriskhanova 2018]; 3) the use of shot freezeframe and shot repetition to appeal to the viewer's attention [Mulvey 2006].

TV interviews [Ilie 1999] may be categorized as live interviews, which are usually face-to-face interaction between people; phone interviews, the events where the interlocutors do not see each other; recorded interviews which can be both face-to-face or done via phone, but they are not real time interviews and are conducted beforehand. TV interviews are a part of journalistic interviews which are based on the information acquisition (in comparison to social studies where they are viewed as means of polling). A TV interview is a type of TV discourse which occurs in the situation of interpersonal communication, conditioned by mass communication and institutional status of the interviewee [Yashina 2007]. Since we address DISCOURSE SPACE construal in narratives which constitute a part of face-to-face TV interviews, we specify the plot structure and viewer engagement structure which appear in this media discourse format. Considering the interview plot structure explored in discursive studies, we can specify its characteristics: 1) distinct roles of interview participants [Milroy 1987; Wolfson 1976]; 2) hybrid discourse types which appear in the plot implementation (expository, rhetorical, and echo) [Ilie 1999]; 3) two distinct communication regimes which are sharing and obtaining information [Scherbatyh 2016]; 4) a combination of staged character and spontaneity [Yashina 2007].

Concerning the viewer engagement structure characteristics, the studies name the following ones: 1) high dynamicity in answering and resisting interviewee's questions [Clayman, Heritage 2002]; 2) a significant power disbalance in favor of the interviewers, as they can choose topics and formulate questions, whereas the interviewees have to follow those questions and are obliged to answer them [Milroy 1987]; 3) a variety of cooperative strategies applied during the interaction which are commonly studied following Grice's cooperative principles [Molenaar, Smit 1996]; 4) multimodal alignment traced in both verbal and nonverbal behavior and explored via contextualization cues which allow to interpret the interactional moves and semantic content [Gumperz 1982].

Overall, DISCOURSE SPACE construal in two media formats manifests several common characteristics; still, there are differences in both plot structure and viewer engagement structure which appear in speech and gesture use. To explore them, we address the ways multimodal deixis representing DISCOURSE SPACE can be expressed in speech and gesture.

Multimodal deixis in gesture and speech

In the recent studies gestures have been regarded not solely as random motions but as a fully functioning level of communication in discourse. They can be regarded as signs that people give to their interlocutors and themselves while producing speech [Bavelas et al. 2008; Chu, Kita 2011; Cienki 2017]. DISCOURSE SPACE construal is commonly explored via deictic semantics [Levinson 2003; Majid et al. 2004; Shusterman, Li 2016] which has developed the research methods applicable to both gesture and speech; therefore, they allow to study multimodal deixis in both media discourse formats. Following Levinson (2003), four major deictic coordinates which can stimulate the coordination of speech and gesture, are identified; they are 1) closer to the speaker, 2) farther from the speaker, 3) pointing to the discourse space of communication, 4) pointing to the discourse space not present in the discourse space of communication. Additionally, there are studies addressing the problem of speech deixis [Paducheva 2008; Borisova, Obchinnikova 2011; Apresjan 2014] which identify the discursive markers of deictic meanings and which may be consequently exploited to study the speech and gesture alignment via the semantics of discursive markers contingent on gesture types. There are also studies which address the problem of deixis in gestures, most commonly in deictic gestures as contingent on discursive schemata or the patterns of discourse components construal [Clark 2003; Enfield et al. 2007; Le Guen 2011; Cooperrider 2017].

These three approaches allow to address different research questions with the foci on deictic coordinates variation across languages, discourses and cultures in the first approach, on gesture variation as dependent on discursive markers in the second approach, and speech and gesture alignment as maintained by discursive schemata. Since in this study we explore DISCOURSE SPACE construal in two media discourse formats which perform a) different "plot structures" and b) different perspective or "viewer engagement" structures via discursive schemata, we adopt the third approach and will explore the speech and gesture alignment in deictic gestures as coordinated (synchronized) with discursive schemata in the contrastive analysis of film and TV narratives plot structures and viewer engagement structures.

The discourse narratives in film and TV interview narratives, therefore, manifest two types of discursive schemata, with their first type revealing the plot contents, and the other revealing the viewer engagement perspective. Since the plot contents may be explored via the schemata representing the discourse types, in this study we address the discursive schemata of argumentation and description which can be contrasted in both film and TV interview narratives. Regarding argumentative discourse, the studies account for its potential in expressing opinions and beliefs [Amossy 2009], subjectivity and intersubjec-tivity [von Stutterheim, Klein 1989], as well as argumentation schemata Example, Cause to Effect and Effect to Cause, Practical Reasoning, Inconsistency [Cabrio et al. 2013]. The studies of descriptive discourse commonly address discourse themes [Merlo, Mansur

2004], discourse components and descriptive event types [Longacre 1996]. These studies allow to identify the discursive schemata which could be contrasted as contingent on deictic gestures in film and TV interview narratives.

Addressing viewer engagement structures, we appeal to the discursive schemata manifesting the rhetorical structure of discourse. Following the seminal paper of Mann and Thompson (1988) revealing these schemata and the study of Kibrik and Podlesskaya (2009) who integrate them into multimodal (speech and gesture as well as in other modalities) research, we consider these discursive schemata as modulating both film and TV interview narratives. These include Emphasizing opinion or assessment, Self-correcting, Specification, Generalization, Initializing communication, Chain of arguments and other schemata.

Coordination patterns of deictic gestures

and deictic discursive schemata in speech

Deictic gestures which are examined in the current paper can fall under the Pierce's notion of indexes, dynamic signs which form a connection between the speaker (origo) and the target of the vector created by such gestures [Clark 2003]. These gestures are a part of a deictic field that is formed around the interlocutors, thus hereby we will call such gestures deictic. Deictic gestures vary in forms, palm orientation, movement and the position that hands have in space. These properties of deictic gestures might depend on culture [Enfield 2001; Kita 2009] and information that is being conveyed [Enfield et al. 2007]. Deictic gestures can be divided into two categories according to the precision, type of the referent, form and movement of the hand. The first type, pointing gestures are regarded as the standard representative of deictic gestures. They are vectors, created in space by some body part, a line, that connects the speaker and the referent [Clark 2003; Kita 2003]. Their various forms are also context dependent. Index figure pointing is used in situations when some clarification is needed while hand and thumb pointing occur when little precision is needed [Wilkins 2003]. Another factor which might influence the form is the distance between the gesturer and the referent: further located objects are addressed with the whole hand whereas closer objects might require precision in order to be distinguished from the surroundings and the speaker might use an index finger. The same choice of a pointing gesture may be true for describing situations and events, depending on their proximity [Cochet, Vauclair 2014]. There are several typologies of pointing gestures, depending on the referent: they can be abstract and concrete [McNeil 1992] or entity- / place- / action-referring [Cooperrider 2017]. In addition, pointing gestures might perform three functions. The first is imperative pointing which expresses a request for an object or an action. The second is declarative expressive pointing which is used to share interest in an object/ event with the interlocutor, and the last declarative informative pointing which is used to provide information for the other party [Camaioni 1997; Tomasello et al. 2007; Cochet, Vauclair 2014]. However, pointing is not the only type of deictic gestures. Another type, touching or also termed placing (see: [Clark 2003]), is also a deictic gesture which can have various forms and occur in various contexts determining its meaning. Whereas the pointing gesture creates a vector in space, the touching gesture locates an object/notion in space [Clark 2003].

Still, pointing and touching gestures relate to the functional gesture use and do not directly relate to their discursive meanings. This explains the need to identify the dis-

cursive meanings of deictic gestures explicit via speech as manifested by their functional types. In the study performed by Le Guen (2011), three discursive schemata of deictic gesture meaning are presented. The first schema is Object reference type which can be direct, metonymic, and metaphoric. Direct pointing to actual places is manifested when the arm is oriented according to the accurate angle with respect to the target (i. e., the actual place occupied by the entity in the world) and when the target is available to speakers even in case it is not visible. Metonymic pointing is found when a speaker points, for instance, to an empty chair someone has just left to speak about this person. In Metaphorical pointing (or abstract pointing by McNeill (1992)), the target is metaphorically related to a person or place. The second discursive schema is Move type which comprises Path indication and Placement indication. In Path indication the speaker's extended arm indicates the direction of the target with a focus on manner or direction of move. In Placement indication the target is the focal attention point which will be accentuated in speech. The third schema is Frame of reference type [Levinson 2003] which can refer to either Speech event or Narrated event. Narrated event is observed when the objects or participants are performing some actions which do not correspond to the speech event, for instance if the speaker is talking about a target located in a distant place or when the speaker describes the event which follows the event in speech but cannot occur in the existing setting [Flud-ernik 1996].

Presumably, while exploring the coordination of discursive schemata in speech and the discursive schemata manifested in deictic gestures, we will identify the specific patterns of multimodal deixis in film and TV interview narratives and contrast these patterns to reveal the specifics of two media formats.

Methods and procedure

In this section, we present the research data and the method used to explore the multimodal deixis, which is the multimodal analysis of discursive schemata coordination patterns in speech and deictic gesture occurring in film and TV interview narratives.

The research data are 10 film narratives of 10 highly ranked actors of 1960-1980s (5 actresses and 5 actors). These are A. Freindlikh ("Stalker", 1979), L. Akhedzhakova ("Garage", 1979), L. Gurchenko ("Love and Doves", 1984), N. Mordyukova ("Relatives", 1981), V. Alentova ("Time for thought", 1982), S. Bondarchuk ("Man's fate", 1959), R. Bykov ("The Doorbell Rings, Open the Door", 1961), V. Tikhonov ("Let Us Live Till Monday", 1968), Yu. Nikulin (from "They Fought for the Country", 1975), A. Batalov ("Moscow Does not Believe in Tears", 1980); and 10 TV interview narratives presented by the same actors and actresses where the interviews with V. Bykov, Yu. Nikulin, and V. Tikhonov were taken in the studio, the interview with S. B ondarchuk was taken in his study, and the interview with A. Batalov was taken on the river embankment. As we have shown, to explore multimodal deixis, we will identify the co-speech synchronization of two deictic gesture types, pointing and touching. In Fig. 1, there are two examples manifesting the use of these gestures in film and TV interview.

In Fig. 1 a, R. Bykov performing in "The doorbell rings, open the door" is using a touching deictic gesture which is synchronized in speech with /я ошибаюсь/ ("I am mistaken"); the referent of deixis is the speaker himself. In Figure 1b, the same actor performing in the TV interview is using a pointing gesture synchronized in speech with /это

Fig. 1. Deictic gestures in media narratives

Fig. 2. Discursive schemata of multimodal DISCOURSE SPACE construal in media narratives

была первая барменша бара/ ("that was the best barwoman of the bar"); the referent of deixis is a woman who is not a participant of the speech event.

In Fig. 2, we present the methodological framework developed for the study basing on the works exploring a) DISCOURSE SPACE construal in film and television interview narratives, b) multimodal deixis in gesture and speech, and c) coordination patterns of deictic gestures and deictic discursive schemata in speech presented in the previous section.

Hence, in the study we explore three types of deictic gesture schemata — Object reference, Move, and Frame of reference, as contingent on the speech schemata — Plot contents, and Viewer engagement, in two media formats, film and TY interview.

In Table 1, we present the taxonomy of discursive schemata employed for studying speech behavior in film and IT interview narratives. Since the TV interviews employ two discourse types, argumentation and description, we give the discursive schemata for each of them separately.

To identify which discursive schema/schemata the actor is using in speech, we address the discursive markers appearing in every clause in speech.

In Table 2, we present the taxonomy of discursive schemata employed for studying deictic gesture behavior in film and IT interview narratives.

Table 1. Discursive schemata of plot contents and viewer engagement in speech

Argumentation Description

Discursive schemata revealing plot contents Discursive schemata revealing plot contents

Opinion Achievement

(Emotional) assessment Process

Stating reasons, consequences, conditions State

Contrast Accentuated subject

Accusation Accentuated object

Agreement/disagreement Accentuated action or state

Appeal to action Accentuated characteristics

Promise Accentuated time

Threat Accentuated place

Comparison —

Appeal to power —

Discursive schemata revealing viewer engagement Discursive schemata revealing viewer engagement

Emphasizing opinion or assessment Emphasizing discourse component

Self-correcting Self-correcting

Specification Specification

Generalization Generalization

Intersubjectivity Chain of events

Appeal to attention New event

Rhetorical communication Appeal to attention

Initializing communication Self-quote

Chain of arguments Quoting others

Self-quote Figurativity

Quoting others —

Figurativity —

Table 2. Discursive schemata of Object reference, Move and Frame of reference in deictic gesture

Object reference Move Frame of reference

Discursive schemata Discursive schemata Discursive schemata

Direct pointing Path indication Speech event

Metonymic pointing Placement indication Narrated event

Metaphoric pointing — —

To identify which discursive scheme/schemata the actor is using in gesture, we address the two deictic gesture types, pointing and touching, manifesting Object reference, Move and Frame of reference [Le Guen 2011]; the schemata were defined as modulated by the contents of speech.

Data analysis

In this section, we present the analysis algorithm of speech and gesture schemata identification in the recorded data and also describe the research steps to explore multimodal deixis in film and TV interview.

To identify which discursive schema/schemata the actor is using in speech, we address the discursive markers appearing in every clause in speech.

For instance, in Argumentation plot contents Opinion schema appears in /это чуть ли не единственный случай в мировой истории/ ("this might be the only case in world history"), (Emotional) assessment appears in /люблю свою работу/ ("I love my job"), Accusation is found in /у вас сердца никакого нету/ ("you have no heart"). Argumentation viewer engagement Emphasizing opinion or assessment schema appears in /конечно, я ему очень благодарен за это, безусловно/ ("of course I am very grateful to him for it no doubt"), Self-correcting schema is found in /если я не ошибаюсь/ ("if I am not mistaken").

In Description plot contents Achievement schema is identified in /а наши взяли этого Ганса и перекинули его через эту дорогу/ ("and our (troops) caught this Hans and threw him across this road"), Process schema appears in /это мы ведь играли не их/ ("we were not playing them"), Accentuated subject is found in /так что не все хотят быть руководителями/ ("not everyone wants to be a director"), Accentuated time appears in /год их не видел/ ("it has been a year since I last saw them").

Description viewer engagement Generalization schema can be shown with /ну вот так это было/ ("this is the way it happened"), Chain of events schema — with /хохотали лежали и плакали/ ("[we] were all on the floor in stitches"), Appeal to attention schema — with /знаешь / он попятился попятился/ ("you know he moved back back").

To give an example of deictic gesture use, in Fig. 3 a, b, we show the actors employing these gestures in the construal of DISCOURSE SPACE in film and TV interview narratives.

In Fig. 3 a (film narrative), actor V. Tikhonov is using a pointing gesture directing it onto the students. He is talking about the letters which he wants his students to address in /а потом были только письма / сотни писем / читайте их / они опубликованы/ ("And then there were only letters, hundreds of letters, read them, they are published"). Importantly, that while pointing at the students, the actor is speaking about the action he wants his students to perform since the main phase of the gesture is synchronized with читайте ("read"); therefore, this gesture is clearly a Metonymic pointing. Meanwhile, the gesture does not indicate any path of action (the way the action should be performed) but indicates the addressees whom the actor wishes to perform this action; consequently, this is Placement indication. Since the actor is talking about the action which might start since the moment of the present event, involves the present event participants into its performance, this is a Speech event.

In Fig. 3 b, actress V. Alentova in a TV interview is using a touching gesture while specifying the reason of the event and stresses it by saying и это было то (and it was something that). Hence, this is a Direct pointing. Still, the gesture does not indicate any

и это было ТО/and it was SOMETHING THAT

Fig. 3. Deictic gesture use in film and TV interview narratives

path of action; therefore, this is Placement indication. The event the actress is pointing at is clearly not the event which might take place in the studio; consequently, this is a Narrated event.

The research procedure involved several steps.

At Step 1, we annotated the research corpus of film and TV interview narratives adopting the inventory of discursive schemata of argumentation and description in speech and discursive schemata embodied in the deictic gestures (we also annotated other functional gesture types as was shown in [Kiose et al. 2022]). Annotation was made in ELAN in several tiers allowing to identify the synchronization patterns of speech and gesture discursive schemata.

At Step 2, we identified the distribution of discursive schemata in speech and gesture in two subcorpora, film narratives and TV interview narratives, and also in the individual actor's narrative. This allowed 1) to identify the significant differences in the use of discursive schemata in speech and gesture in film and TV narratives, 2) to contrast the use of co-speech deictic gesture as modulated by discursive schemata in argumentation and description in film and TV narratives, 3) to find the significant distinctions in pointing and touching gesture use with argumentation and description in film and TV narratives. A series of one-way ANOVA tests in Jamovi statistical software was run for the purpose.

At Step 3, we explore the distribution of discursive schemata in speech and gesture in film narratives and TV interview narratives as modulated by the Plot contents and Viewer engagement structure characteristics earlier disclosed as typical of film narratives and TV interview narratives.

Results and discussion

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Step 1. To demonstrate the annotation procedure, we will illustrate a fragment from V. Alentova film narrative (Fig. 4). To identify the gesture types and the discursive schemata in speech, we employed a coded system with deictic gestures coded as 301 (pointing) and 302 (touching).

Fig. 4. ELAN Annotation sample

We may observe that at the moment of producing either of two clauses /значит смотри/ (well, look) and /у Сережи в субботу день рождения/ ("Serezha has birthday on Saturday") Alentova employs a Touching deictic gesture <302> produced by both hands. While uttering these clauses, Alentova enumerates the micro events which are to happen in the near future. Still, to introduce this enumeration, she uses her left hand as if manifesting the event which she wants the interlocutor to observe; therefore, this deictic gesture is clearly a Metaphorical pointing. At the same time with the forefinger of her right hand Alentova is indicating the direction towards this palm-event which she wants the interlocutor to follow; this is then a Path indication schema. Since she is introducing this event-to-follow while pronouncing the clause appealing to the interlocutor's attention in /значит смотри/ ("well, look"), we consider it a manifestation of Speech event. Still, in producing the second clause /у Сережи в субботу день рождения/ ("Serezha has birthday on Saturday") Alentova touches her left-hand finger with the forefinger of her right hand to start enumerating the microevents-to-follow. This deictic gesture is clearly a Metaphoric emblematic pointing where a finger represents a microevent. Still, in this case this is a Placement indication since the meta-phoric microevent is held on her open palm. This microevent does not involve the participants and setting represented in speech; therefore, it can be referred to as Narrated event.

Alentova employs several argumentation and description schemata in speech. In / значит смотри/ ("well, look") she uses Appeal to action as a Plot contents schema and Appeal to attention as a Viewer engagement schema. In /у Сережи в субботу день рождения/ ("Serezha has birthday on Saturday") we find Stating reasons, consequences, conditions as a Plot contents schema and Specification as a Viewer engagement schema manifesting argumentation. Additionally, this clause manifests description discursive schemata of State and Accentuated subject as Plot contents schemata.

The same procedure was applied to annotate all the clauses in the actors' speech with gestures. The total number of the clauses which co-occur with deictic gestures in film

Table 3. Discursive schemata of Object reference, Move and Frame of reference in media discourse

Discursive schemata Film narrative TV interview narrative

Object reference

Direct pointing 23 18

Metonymic pointing 16 4

Metaphoric pointing 18 37

Move

Path indication 4 22

Placement indication 51 36

Frame of reference

Speech event 24 8

Narrated event 32 51

narratives was 54; the total number of the clauses appearing with deictic gestures in TV interview narratives was 59.

Step 2. In Table 3, we present the total number of discursive schemata found in gesture of film and TV narratives.

We may observe that gesture discursive schemata in film and TV narratives manifest differences which appear in all types of schemata. Still the Paired samples T-test shows these differences are statistically insignificant with Student's t (6) = -0.18 at p = 0.863. Additionally, we presume that 1) the differences (although not significant) can occur due to various speech discursive schemata applied in the clauses, 2) there may be distinctions in the individual speech and gesture multimodal deixis. Therefore, we further addressed individual participants gesture behavior to determine the gesture and speech contingencies.

First, we contrasted the use of discursive schemata in speech and gesture in film and TV narratives. Two One-Way ANOVA (Welch's) tests were performed for this purpose with a) speech schemata used with deictic gesture as determined by film or TV interview, b) gesture schemata as determined by film or TV interview.

There were 42 speech schemata tested as potentially contingent on either film or TV interview; still, only 6 schemata displayed significant differences in their use with a media format. In argumentation these include the discursive schemata revealing Plot contents, which are Stating reasons, consequences, conditions with F = 4.59 at p = 0.034, Contrast with F = 4.43 at p = 0.039; also, they include one discursive schema revealing Viewer engagement, which is Quoting others with F = 4.67 at p = 0.034. In description these include the discursive schema revealing Plot contents, which is Accentuated place with F = 17.16 at p < 0.001; also, they include two discursive schemata revealing Viewer engagement, which are Specification with F = 17.21 at p < 0.001, and Chain of events with F = 9.53 at p = 0.003.

The results show that the most significant differences are observed in description schemata, and mostly relate to specifying information and stressing the place; in both cases the schemata with deictic gestures are found more frequently in TV interviews.

There were 7 gesture schemata tested as potentially contingent on either film or TV interview. Noticeably, 6 out of them manifested significant distinctions with an only exception of Direct pointing. In Table 4, we present the One-Way ANOVA (Welch's) test results for all gesture schemata.

Table 4. Differences in Object reference, Move and Frame of reference

Discursive schemata Film vs TV interview, F; p

Object reference

Direct pointing 1.77; 0.187

Metonymic pointing 10.39; 0.002

Metaphoric pointing 10.49; 0.002

Move

Path indication 16.76; < .001

Placement indication 21.95; < .001

Frame of reference

Speech event 14.28; < .001

Narrated event 11.24; 0.001

As the results show, the strongest differences are found in the use of Move and Frame of reference schemata. The diagrams in Figs 4-6 allow to determine how these differences are distributed in film vs. TV interview.

Fig. 5 shows the distribution in gesture Object reference.

In Object reference, the differences were found in Metonymic and Metaphoric pointing; however, the diagrams show that while the number of Metaphoric and Metonymic pointing in film is almost the same, it is not the same in TV interviews. Whereas Meto-nymic pointing is infrequent in interviews, it is the Metaphoric pointing which is commonly found.

In Move schemata, the tendency in the Path and Placement indication distribution is similar; still, Placement indication occurs significantly more often in films.

In Fig. 7, we present the distribution in gesture Frame of reference.

We determine that in film, Speech events synchronized with deictic gestures appear more frequently; presumably, this happens due to higher involvement of actors into the situation of the film shot, while in the TV interview it is the narrated event which the actors accentuate by deictic gesturing.

Next, we explored the use of co-speech deictic gesture as modulated by discursive schemata in argumentation and description in both film and TV narratives. To do it, we performed two One-Way ANOVA (Welch's) tests with speech schemata as modulated by a) pointing gestures and b) touching gestures. It is to be mentioned that only one speech schema was used with deictic gestures which manifested significant distinctions in its use with pointing and touching gestures, that was Accentuated characteristics, with F = 4.72 at p = 0.045 for pointing gestures, and F = 4.79 at p = 0.034 for touching gestures. Accentuated characteristics schema appears significantly more frequently with pointing gestures than with touching gestures, with 21 and 2 cases in films, and 15 and 7 cases in TV interview, correspondingly.

Step 3. The obtained results describing multimodal deixis in film and interview helped specify the earlier described characteristics of these two media formats. Below, we will consider them separately.

Multimodal deixis in film manifested the preference for Metonymic pointing, Placement indication and Speech event construal. Importantly, we did not observe enhanced embodiment role appearing in gesturing in films [Kristeva 2014] in terms of deictic gestures, as opposed to TV interviews. This might be explained by the fact that the interview

0 1

c

Metaphoric pointing

Fig. 5. Object reference

participants were also actors; however, there is a more potent argument that these differences will appear in other gesture types, for instance, in pragmatic or representational gestures [Cienki, Mittelberg 2013]. Another finding was that Metaphoric deictic gestures which were expected to appear more often in films did not manifest this tendency. Importantly, there were cases (see Fig. 7) of entrenched or emblematic multimodal metonymies and metaphors expressed in speech and gesture behavior of actors performing in film.

In Fig. 8 a, Yu. Nikulin uses a pointing gesture to show the direction to God who is commonly deemed to be somewhere up; hence, we observe a metonymic shift from subject onto the direction towards this subject. In Fig. 8 b, L. Akhedzhakova points at each of the event participants while touching with a forefinger of her left hand one of the fingers of her right hand, which is an emblematic gesture of enumeration; still, this is not a case of sole enumeration but also focusing attention on each particular participant. Therefore, there is a metaphoric shift from the finger she is touching to a participant.

Fig. 6. Move

Fig. 7. Frame of reference

Presumably, the prevalence of Metonymic pointing is interconnected with another frequently appearing gesture schema which is Speech event construal. Since this is the fictive event with an emphasis on emotionality, imagery, pragmaticity and logics [Ivanov 1976] which the actor is performing in the film shot, the components of the shot are located in close proximity with the speaker and there is no need to point at them by forming an iconic view of them. Additionally, other gesture types (for instance, representational) might produce the exaggerated gesturing of "look into the camera" type [Ciccognani 2018].

Multimodal deixis in TV interview displayed the significant prevalence of Met-aphoric pointing, Path indication and Narrative event construal. In terms of speech

a b

Не дай БОГ ребята услышат / Обсудить КАЖДОГО ПАЙЩИКА

GOD forbid the guys to hear в отдельности / to discuss EVERY

SHAREHOLDER individually

Fig. 8. Emblematic multimodal metonymy and metaphor with deictic gesture in film

возьми эту / take that one возьми ее / take her

Fig. 9. Metaphoric deictic gesture in TV interview

schemata specifics, we determined the significant prevalence of Specifying information and Accentuating the place. Since in the interview, the speaker focuses in the Narrated event, we expected higher dynamicity in answering the interviewee's questions [Clay-man, Heritage 2002], which was found in both speech and in the use of Metaphoric deictic gesturing.

Fig. 9 shows an example of Metaphoric gesturing which clearly complies with the Narrated event.

и наши вправо / ...and our (troops) to the right

Fig. 10. Narrated event in TV interview

In Fig. 9 the actor points at a person who is not nearby. The gesture is used as if playing out a situation when the individual that they are describing was in the same room with the actor to intensify the argumentation. These findings show that multimodal deixis is mediated by the iconicity in gesturing of the communicators (Metaphoric pointing), which proves that it reflects two communication regimes of sharing and obtaining information [Scherbatyh 2016].

The prevalence of Path indication in deictic gesturing evidences in favour of the speakers' high involvement into a narrative event construal; presumably this happens since the speakers are professional actors and produce a combination of staged character and spontaneity [Yashina 2007] with exaggerated staged effects. Importantly, that acting out a narrated event might have influenced the speech schemata choice. To bring closer the components of the plot implementation [Ilie 1999], the speakers focalize in speech the details of the event, accentuate the place of the event. This involvement is manifested in Fig. 10.

In Fig. 10, we observe the example of a touching gesture, where the actor abstractly "touches" the sides which the troops moved to. This quite frequent identification of the sides (right-left, west-east) is implemented by a pointing gesture, for instance pointing to the right while showing the orientation in space. The co-speech gesture chosen by the actor intensifies the Placement and Path indication of the movement of the object in the Narrated event.

Overall, multimodal deixis contributes to intensifying specific characteristics of plot contents and viewer engagement in two media formats, film and TV interview. Most notably, these are the characteristics related to their different narrative regimes and different combination of staged performance and spontaneity.

Conclusions

In this study, we addressed the problem of multimodal information construal in contrasting two most common media formats, film and TV interviews, in short narratives produced by the same actors. We presumed that since these two formats display differ-

ences in their plot content and viewer engagement characteristics, the latter might affect the way the actors employ speech and gesture to present the discourse space. With the research data of 20 film and TV interview fragments with 10 highly rated Russian actors (5 men and 5 women) we developed a three-stage procedure to explore the possible contingency of discursive schemata presenting discourse space in speech and of discursive schemata presenting object of reference, move and frame of reference in gesture.

The results manifest that multimodal deixis contributes to intensifying both types of these characteristics. Importantly, the results contributed to specifying several of them. For instance, we did not observe enhanced embodiment role appearing in gesturing in films in terms of deictic gestures, as opposed to TV interviews. We also identified higher dynamicity in responsive narratives in TV interviews, which was found in both speech and in the use of metaphoric deictic gesturing. Statistical results show that while multimodal deixis in film manifests the significant preference for metonymic pointing, placement indication and speech event construal; in TV interview we observe the prevalence of metaphoric pointing, path indication and narrative event construal. Additionally, TV interviews display the speech schemata which specify information and accentuate the place. Overall, the study shows that there are two basic types of discourse characteristics which produce higher differences between film and TV interview formats. These are different narrative regimes and different combination of staged performance and spontaneity.

References

Agamben, G. (2019). Notes on Gesture. In C. Kul-Want (Ed.), Philosophers on Film from Bergson to Badiou:

A Critical Reader (pp. 208-217). New York Chichester, West Sussex: Columbia University Press. Amossy, R. (2009). Argumentation in discourse: A socio-discursive approach to arguments. Informal Logic, 3 (29), 252-267.

Apresjan, V Yu. (2014). There, here and now. On temporal meanings of spatial deictic words. Russkii iazyk

v nauchnom osveshchenii, 1 (27), 9-41. (In Russian) Auerbach, J. (2007). Body shots: Early cinema's incarnations. Berkeley: University of California Press. Barthes, R. (1981). Camera lucida: Reflections on photography. New York: Hill & Wang. Bavelas, J., Gerwing, J., Sutton, C., Prevost, D. (2008). Gesturing on the telephone: Independent effects of

dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language, 2 (58), 495-520. Borisova, Е. G., Ovchinnikova, T. Е. (2011). Parameter of nearness in the metaphorical space. In A E. Kibrik (Ed.), Komp'iuternaia lingvistika i intellektual'nye tekhnologii: po materialam ezhegodnoi Mezhdun-arodnoi konferentsii "Dialog" (Bekasovo, 25-29 maia 2011 g.), 10 (17) (pp. 153-158). Moscow: RGGU Publ. (In Russian)

Bugaeva, L. D. (2016). The language of emotions in film. Media Linguistics, 2 (12), 61-70. (In Russian) Cabrio, E., Tonelli, S., Villata, S. (2013). From discourse analysis to argumentation schemes and back: Relations and differences. In J. Leite, T. Cao Son, P. Torroni, L. Torre, S. Woltran (Eds), Computational Logic in Multi-Agent Systems: 14th International Workshop, CLIMA XIV, Corunna, Spain, September 16-18, 2013, Proceedings (pp. 1-17). Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer. Camaioni, L. (1997). The emergence of intentional communication in ontogeny, phylogeny and pathology.

European Psychologist, 2 (3), 216-225. Chare, N., Watkins, L. (Eds) (2017). Gesture and film: Signalling new critical perspectives. Abingdon; New York: Routledge.

Chu, M., Kita, S. (2011). The nature of gestures' beneficial role in spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1 (140), 102. Ciccognani, M. (2018). Metacinematic gestures: An investigation of the productionist aspect of self-reflexive

films. Dr. Sci. thesis. Leicester. Cienki, A. (2017). Ten lectures on spoken language and gesture from the perspective of cognitive linguistics: Issues of dynamicity and multimodality. Leiden; Boston: Brill.

Cienki, A., Mittelberg, I. (2013). Creativity in the forms and functions of spontaneous gestures with speech. In T. Veale, K. Feyaerts, C. Forceville, Creativity and the Agile Mind: A Multi-Disciplinary Study of a Multi-Faceted Phenomenon (pp. 231-252). Berlin; Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.

Clark, H. H. (2003). Pointing and placing. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 243-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Clayman, S., Heritage, J. (2002). The news interview: Journalists and public figures on the air. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cochet, H., Vauclair, J. (2014). Deictic gestures and symbolic gestures produced by adults in an experimental context: Hand shapes and hand preferences. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 3 (19), 278-301.

Cooperrider, K. (2017). Foreground gesture, background gesture. Gesture, 2 (16), 176-202.

Couldry, N. (2002). Media rituals: A critical approach. London: Routledge.

Deleuze, G. (1986). Cinema 1: The Movement Image. London: Athlone Press.

Deleuze, G. (1989). Cinema 2: The Time-Image. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

Djonov, E., Zhao, S. (Eds.). (2013). Critical Multimodal Studies of Popular Discourse. New York: Routledge.

Dobrosklonskaya, T. G. (2016). Methods of analyzing video-verbal texts. Media Linguistics, 2 (12), 13-25. (In Russian)

Eisenstein, S. M. (1964-1968). Selected works. In 6 vols. Moscow: Iskusstvo Publ. (In Russian)

Enfield, N. J. (2001). 'Lip-pointing': A discussion of form and function with reference to data from Laos. Gesture, 1 (2), 185-211.

Enfield, N. J., Kita, S., De Ruiter, J. P. (2007). Primary and secondary pragmatic functions of pointing gestures. Journal of Pragmatics, 10 (39), 1722-1741.

Fludernik, M. (1996). Towards a 'Natural'Narratology. London: Routledge.

Gumperz, J. (1982). Discourse Strategies (Studies in Interactional Sociolinguistics). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Harbord, J. P. (2015). Agamben's cinema: Psychology versus an ethical form oflife. Necsus. European Journal of Media Studies, 4, 13-30.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Ilie, C. (1999). Question-response argumentation in talk shows. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 975-999.

Iriskhanova, O. K. (2018). On the issue of narration dynamics evaluation. In V. V. Feschenko (Ed.), Obrazy iazyka i zigzagi diskursa: sbornik nauchnykh statei k 70-letiiu V. Z. Dem'iankova (pp. 192-215). Moscow: Kul'turnaia revoliutsiia Publ. (In Russian)

Iriskhanova, O. K. (2019). Cognitive poetics of gestures. Cognitive Studies of Language, 36, 72-86.

Iriskhanova, O. K., Cienki, A. (2018). The semiotics of gestures in cognitive linguistics: Contributions and challenges. Issues in Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 25-36.

Iriskhanova, O. K., Kiose, M. I., Leonteva A. V., Agafonova, O. V. (2022). Multimodal spatial deixis in speech and gestures: frames of reference in explanatory discourse. Issues of Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 17-31.

Ivanov, V. V. (1976). Outline on the history of semiotics in the USSR. Moscow: Nauka Publ. (In Russian)

Kibrik, A. A. (2009). Mode, genre and other parameters of discourse classification. Voprosy iazykoznaniia, 2, 3-21. (In Russian)

Kibrik, A. A., Podlesskaja, V. I. (2009). Night Dream Stories: A Corpus Study of spoken Russian discourse. Moscow: IaSK Publ. (In Russian)

Kiose, M., Leonteva, A., Agafonova, O. (2022). Aesthetic multimodality of speech and gesture: Towards its functional framework. Languages and Modalities, 2, 1-17.

Kita, S. (2003). Pointing: A foundational building block of human communication. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 1-8). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Kita, S. (2009). Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and cognitive processes, 2 (24), 145-167.

Kita, S., Ôzyùrek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 1 (48), 16-32.

Kristeva, J. (2014). La révolution du langage poétique. Lavant-garde à la fin du XIXe siècle: Lautréamont et Mallarmé. Paris: Média Diffusion.

Kubryakova, E. S., Aleksandrova, O. V. (1997). Types of space in text and discourse. In E. S. Kubryakova, O. V. Aleksandrova (Eds), Kategorizatsiia mira: prostranstvo i vremia: materialy nauchnoi konferentsii (pp. 15-25). Moscow: Dialog-MGU Publ. (In Russian)

Lavender, A. (2021). Multimodal Acting and Performing. In L. Ellestrom (Ed.), Beyond Media Borders. Vol. 1: Intermedial Relations among Multimodal Media (pp. 113-140). Cham: Palgrave Macmillan.

Le Guen, O. (2011). Modes of pointing to existing spaces and the use of frames of reference. Gesture, 3 (11), 271-307.

Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Language.

Longacre, R. E. (1996). The grammar of discourse. New York: Springer.

Machin, D. (2013). What is multimodal critical discourse studies? Critical Discourse Studies, 4 (10), 347-355.

Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B., Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3 (8), 108-114.

Mann, W. C., Thompson, S. A. (1988). Rhetorical structure theory: Toward a functional theory of text organization. Text-interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of Discourse, 3 (8), 243-281.

Masterman, L. (1997). A Rationale for Media Education. In R. Kubey (Ed.), Media Literacy around the World (pp. 15-68). New York: Routlege.

McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Merlo, S., Mansur, L. L. (2004). Descriptive discourse: topic familiarity and disfluencies. Journal of Communication Disorders, 6 (37), 489-503.

Mesh, K., Cruz, E., van de Weijer, J., Burenhult, N., Gullberg, M. (2021). Effects of Scale on Multimodal Deixis: Evidence from Quiahije Chatino. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 584231.

Milroy, L. (1987). Observing and Analyzing Natural Language: A Critical Account of Sociolinguistic Method. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Mittell, J. (2007). Film and television narrative. In D. Herman (Ed.), The Cambridge Companion to Narrative (pp. 156-171). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Molenaar, N., Smit, J. H. (1996). Asking and answering yes/no questions in survey interviews: a conversational approach. Quality and Quantity, 30, 115-136.

Mulvey, L. (1988). Visual pleasure and narrative cinema. In C. Penley (Ed.), Feminism and Film Theory (pp. 57-68). New York: Routledge.

Mulvey, L. (2006). Death 24x a Second: Stillness and the Moving Image. London: Reaktion Books Ltd.

Noys, B. (2014). Film-of-life: Agamben's profanation of the image. In H. Gustafsson, A. Gr0nstad (Eds), Cinema and Agamben: Ethics, biopolitics and the moving image (pp. 89-102). Bloomsbury: Bloomsbury Academic.

Paducheva, E. V. (2008). Register of interpretation as disambiguating context. In A. E. Kibrik (Ed.), Komp'iuternaia lingvistika i intellektual'nye tekhnologii: po materialam ezhegodnoi Mezhdunarod-noi konferentsii "Dialog" (Bekasovo, 4-8 iiunia 2008 g.), 7 (14) (pp. 412-419). Moscow: RGGU Publ. (In Russian)

Pfeiffer, T. (2010). Understanding multimodal deixis with gaze and gesture in conversational interfaces. Dr. Sci. thesis. Bielefeld.

Plotnikova, S. N. (2011). Discourse space: towards the problem with the definition of the phenomenon. Nauchno-pedagogicheskij zhurnal Vostochnoj Sibiri Magister Dixit, 2 (6), 152-158. (In Russian)

Pronin, A. (2018). Space as a narrative document: Addressing the question of persuasive power of biographic cinematic narrative. In L. D. Bugaeva (Ed.), Prostranstvo i personazh (pp. 198-204). St. Petersburg: Petropolis Publ. (In Russian)

Scherbatyh, E. Ju. (2016). The framework of evaluation semantics in contemporary English interviews. PhD thesis. Yaroslavl. (In Russian)

Smith, M. (2010). Engaging Characters: Further Refectio. In J. Eder, F. Jannidis, R. Schneider (Eds), Characters in Fictional Worlds: Understanding Imaginary Beings in Literature, Film, and Other Media (pp. 232-258). Berlin: De Gruyter.

Shusterman, A., Li, P. (2016). A Framework for Work on Frames of Reference. In D. Barner, A. S. Baron (Eds), Core Knowledge and Conceptual Change (pp. 191-206). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705-722.

Von Stutterheim, C., Klein, W. (1989). Referential movement in descriptive and narrative discourse. North-Holland Linguistic Series: Linguistic Variations, 54, 39-76.

Wilkins, D. (2003). Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms). In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 171-215). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Wolfson, N. (1976). Speech events and natural speech: Some implications for sociolinguistic methodology. Language in Society, 2 (5), 189-209.

Yashina, N. V. (2007). Communicative and intonational specifics of TV-interview discourse (case study of the

American variant of the English language). PhD thesis. Ivanovo. (In Russian) Zykova, I. V (2021). Linguistic creativity in cinematic discourse. In I. V. Zykova (Ed.), Lingvokreativnost' v diskursakh raznykh tipov: Predely i vozmozhnosti (pp. 100-189). Moscow: R. Valent Publ. (In Russian)

Received: September 14, 2023 Accepted: December 13, 2023

Authors' information:

Maria I. Kiose — Dr. Sci. in Philology, Associate Professor, Leading Researcher; maria_kiose@mail.ru

Anna V. Leonteva — PhD in Philology, Research Assistant; lentevanja27@gmail.com Olga V. Agafonova — Junior Researcher; olga.agafonova92@gmail.com

Полимодальный дейксис в медиадискурсе: нарративы кино и телеинтервью*

M. И. Киосе, A. В. Леонтьева, O. В. Агафонова

Московский государственный лингвистический университет, Российская Федерация, 119034, Москва, ул. Остоженка, 38 Институт языкознания РАН,

Российская Федерация, 125009, Москва, Большой Кисловский пер., 1

Для цитирования: Kiose M. I., Leonteva A. V., Agafonova O. V. (2024). Multimodal deixis in media discourse: Film vs TV interview narratives. Медиалингвистика, 11 (1), 30-52. https://doi.org/10.21638/spbu22.2024.103

В статье анализу подвергаются два жанра медиадискурса — кино и телеинтервью, — демонстрирующие специфический характер распределения коммуникативных модальностей речи и жеста в полимодальном дейксисе при конструировании дискурсивного пространства в нарративе. Для установления особенностей этого распределения в работе изучается «содержание сюжета» и «перспектива наблюдателя» как две основные координаты конструирования дискурсивного пространства в медиа. Гипотеза исследования заключается в том, что наличие различий в «содержании сюжета» и «перспективе наблюдателя» в рассматриваемых жанрах будет определять распределение речи и жеста в конструировании дискурсивного пространства актерами. Материалом анализа являются 20 фрагментов кино и телеинтервью с участием рейтинговых российских актеров (5 мужчин и 5 женщин). Основной метод исследования — полимодальный анализ, который применяется для установления паттернов сопряженности (совместной встречаемости) дискурсивных схем, использующихся для конструирования дискурсивного пространства в речи, и дискурсивных схем, служащих для конструирования объекта референции, движения и референциального фрейма в дейк-тических жестах. Результаты показали, что полимодальный дейксис демонстрирует специфические характеристики в кино и телеинтервью. Так, в кино превалирует метонимическое указание, указание на место при конструировании события речи; в то время как в телеинтервью чаще наблюдается метафорическое указание, указание на

* Исследование выполнено в рамках государственного задания «Полимодальный анализ коммуникативного поведения говорящего в разных типах устного дискурса» (проект № 075-03-2020013) в Центре социокогнитивных исследований дискурса в Московском государственном лингвистическом университете.

путь при конструировании события нарратива. Установлено, что в телеинтервью дискурсивные схемы в речи чаще представляют детализированную информацию, а также информацию, уточняющую расположение объекта референции в событии речи или в событии нарратива. В целом исследование позволило обнаружить два основных типа дискурсивных характеристик, определяющих различия в полимодальном дейксисе кино и телеинтервью; к ним отнесены разные нарративные формы и различное сочетание постановочного и спонтанного в поведении актеров. Ключевые слова: кино, телеинтервью, речь, жест, дискурсивное пространство.

Статья поступила в редакцию 14 сентября 2023 г.; рекомендована к печати 13 декабря 2023 г.

Контактная информация:

Киосе Мария Ивановна — д-р филол. наук, доц., вед. науч. сотр.; maria_kiose@mail.ru Леонтьева Анна Васильевна — канд. филол. наук, науч. сотр.; lentevanja27@gmail.com Агафонова Ольга Владимировна — мл. науч. сотр.; olga.agafonova92@gmail.com

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.