Научная статья на тему 'FACTORS OF VARIATION IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION'

FACTORS OF VARIATION IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Текст научной статьи по специальности «Языкознание и литературоведение»

39
7
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Аннотация научной статьи по языкознанию и литературоведению, автор научной работы — Denisov K.M.

Обобщаются взгляды зарубежных исследователей на источники варьирования уровня обучения языку. Первая группа факторов, влияющих на уровень языковой подготовки, включает внешнюю и внутреннюю культуру и языковое окружение. Вторую группу составляют некоторые индивидуальные характеристики студента стилистического, когнитивного, аффективного и демографического характера. Эти факторы могут оказывать взаимное влияние. The article synthesizes views of western researchers on sources of variation in language learning. One key set of factors influencing levels of language acquisition is contextual including: large culture, small culture and foreign language environment. Other crucial components are based on individual student characteristics of stylistic, cognitive and affective, and demographic nature. Both groups may overlap and influence each other

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «FACTORS OF VARIATION IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION»

Библиографический список

1. Психологический словарь / Под ред. В. П. Зинченко, Б. Г. Мещерякова. М., 1999.

2. Тарасова О. Н. Гендерная специфика поведения человека в аспекте взаимодействия вербальных и невербальных компонентов коммуникации: (На материале англоязычных художественных текстов): Дис. ... канд. филол. наук. Иваново, 2006.

3. Leech G. N. Principles of Pragmatics. L.; N. Y., 1983.

4. Macmillan English Dictionary. Bloomsbury Publishing Plc., 2006.

5. Martin A. M. Karen's Little Sister. Scholastic Inc., 1989.

K. M. Denisov

FACTORS OF VARIATION IN LANGUAGE ACQUISITION

Обобщаются взгляды зарубежных исследователей на источники варьирования уровня обучения языку. Первая группа факторов, влияющих на уровень языковой подготовки, включает внешнюю и внутреннюю культуру и языковое окружение. Вторую группу составляют некоторые индивидуальные характеристики студента стилистического, когнитивного, аффективного и демографического характера. Эти факторы могут оказывать взаимное влияние.

The article synthesizes views of western researchers on sources of variation in language learning. One key set of factors influencing levels of language acquisition is contextual including: large culture, small culture and foreign language environment. Other crucial components are based on individual student characteristics of stylistic, cognitive and affective, and demographic nature. Both groups may overlap and influence each other.

Contextual factors of variation in language acquisition include ethnic, national or international background (large culture) and any cohesive social grouping, however small (small culture) [10, p. 240]. It also concerns a foreign language learning environment. According to Kramsch [11, p. 6], culture refers to that which has been grown and groomed (from the Latin colere 'to cultivate') and includes social, historical, and imaginative aspects.

Culture plays a vital role in the formation of the individual's personality and learning processes [2; 14]. Saleh [16], using results from a university study of language learners, asserted that individualism/collectivism (I/C) and cultural tolerance of ambiguity (TO A) affect language learning, mediated by personality types. Individualistic cultures, such as the USA and Western Europe, emphasize loose social ties, autonomy, confrontation, right to privacy, and personal goals, while collectivist cultures (China and Japan) create strong cohesive in-groups with group norms, that value non-confrontation, collaboration, and strong hierarchies and rules.

Large culture and small culture frequently intersect. Students' large-cultural beliefs influence and sometimes limit language achievement in small-cultural university classrooms. Culturally influenced teacher beliefs influence learner beliefs and language learning strategy use. Unspoken or misunderstood differences

© Denisov K. M., 2008

between teacher beliefs and learner beliefs in a specific classroom can cause difficulties and frustration [3]. Fortunately, culturally influenced beliefs of teachers and learners can be modified through indepth discussion and other activities, and such alterations can change behaviour in the language classroom.

Voices from the language classroom [1] reveal the importance of the classroom as a small culture. Other small cultures for language development might include informal or "street" learning and self-access centers. Learners from many countries described their language teachers in metaphoric ways, with categories of metaphors such as Teacher as Hanging Judge, Entertainer, Co-learner, Prophet, Babysitter and Absentee. Analysis according to intimacy and power themes produced three general teaching approaches (autocratic, democratic/participatory, and laissez-affaire). Narratives showed rejection of the first and third approaches and pleasure with the second.

A foreign language (FL) environment is a location where the target language (EFL — as English example) is not the main vehicle of ordinary communication and where input in this language is consequently limited. In foreign language settings, the target language is often taught academically, as a subject to be memorized for tests, rather than as a communication tool, and therefore many foreign language learners have low motivation and poor performance [8].

Among individual student characteristics stylistic factors come first. The term styles refers to general approaches to learning or problem solving, as part of the larger issue of coping with everyday life. Brain hemisphericity, learning styles, and personality types are in this category.

Brain hemisphericity (brain dominance) is the tendency of an individual to process information mainly through either the left hemisphere or the right hemisphere of the brain. Brain activity within and across the two hemispheres is complex, because (a) one hemisphere can sometimes take over certain functions if the other hemisphere is damaged, and (b) gender differences exist in overall brain lateralization (women are more bilateral) and in the corpus callosum, the connective hemisphere-linking brain tissue (which is thicker in women, allowing greater information transfer across hemispheres) [9].

However, some universal generalizations are possible. Both hemispheres carry out the same tasks in different styles: sequential for the left hemisphere and parallel for the right one. Left-hemispheric dominant individuals are more analytic, verbal, linear, logical, and rational learners, whereas, right-hemispheric dominant individuals are more integrative, imagistic (through the visual, tactile, kinesthetic, and auditory senses), nonlinear, intuitive, and emotional learners. Brain hemi-sphericity greatly influences learning styles, academic achievement, and choice of academic major and career path.

Learning styles, such as sensory preferences and field independence (FI) versus field dependence (FD), are the general approaches students use to learn any subject, including another language [7]. Learning style consists of distinctive behaviours, which serve as indicators of how a person learns from and interacts with his or her environment. Taking learning styles into consideration can increase language achievement [15, p. 45].

Sensory preferences include visual, auditory, kinesthetic, and tactile, the last two of which are sometimes clustered into the haptic or hands-on style [6, p. 12]. Visual students enjoy reading, video, computers, pictures, and written classroom instructions and dislike lectures that lack visual support. Auditory students enjoy

purely oral directions, lectures, conversations, and debates. Hands-on or haptic learners like movement, tangible objects, collages, and physical models.

The best known system of personality types is found in the Myers — Briggs Type Indicator, or MBTI [12, p. 25], which contains four dimensions: extroverted/introverted, sensing/intuition, thinking/feeling, and judging/perceiving. The resulting matrix categorizes individuals into sixteen types. Extroverts gain energy from working with others, while introverts gain energy from working alone or with a trusted friend. Sensing-oriented individuals are realistic, practical, and fact-oriented, whereas intuitive individuals are imaginative, futuristic, and theory oriented. Thinking-oriented individuals concern themselves with impersonal analysis and logic, while feeling-oriented individuals are more emotional and overtly compassionate. Judging-oriented people like structure and rapid judgments, while perceiving-oriented individuals are spontaneous and dislike quick decisions.

Cognitive and affective factors include motivation, self-referential judgments, anxiety, and language learning strategies. Each of these is a clear source of variation in language acquisition. Clément, Dornyei, and Noels [4, p. 430] identified five EFL motivational orientations:

1. To make friends and travel.

2. To identify with a target language group.

3. To know various peoples, cultures, and world events.

4. To advance academically or professionally.

5. To understand English-language media.

Students strongly supported all except the second factor; they were not particularly interested in integrating into Anglophone cultures. This model contains three levels. The language level reflects cultural-affective, intellectual, and pragmatic values, associated with a target language and has two subsystems: integrative and instrumental. The learner level concerns fairly stable personality traits of the learner, such as linguistic self-confidence and need for achievement. The learning situation level reflects situation-specific motives and includes course-, teacher-, and group-specific components.

A model by Crookes and Schmidt [5, p. 477] contains the following: (a) interest, (b) relevance, (c) expectancy of success, (d) outcomes, (e) decision to engage in learning, (f) persistence, and (g) high activity level. Tremblay and Gardner [18, p. 516] developed a highly complex, empirically derived model of language learning motivation.

Self-referential judgments, such as self-esteem and self-efficacy, are judgments the learner makes about herself or himself. Attributions and locus of control are also important.

Self-esteem is a judgment of one's own personal worth or value. Global self-esteem arises when the person is around the mental age of eight and is based on two factors: (a) self-perceptions of competence in broad areas, such as academics, sports, social interaction, or physical appearance, and (b) a personal assessment of the importance of these areas. Situational self-esteem relates to a specific setting, event, or activity type. A foreign language student can feel generally good about himself or herself (global self-esteem) but simultaneously experience low situational self-esteem in a negative language learning environment [14, p. 24].

Self-efficacy refers to one's judgments about one's own ability to succeed on a task or long-term (longitudinal) effort. Individuals who doubt their capabilities

might slacken their efforts when facing serious difficulties, but those with strong self-efficacy make greater efforts to master challenges.

Anxiety can either be a state or a permanent trait of fear or apprehension. With debilitating anxiety, motivation suffers, poor performance occurs, and still greater anxiety is aroused, but facilitating anxiety stimulates the learner to try harder and perform better. Certain language activities, such as speaking in front of others or writing a paper, can generate anxiety about performance. Other anxiety causing variables are certain classroom structures, perceived irrelevance of the target language, and culture shock. Language anxiety can be reduced through relaxation, humour, discussion, support groups, and other means.

Learning strategies are steps or operations used by learners to learn more effectively, that is, to facilitate acquisition, storage, retrieval, and use of information. Learning strategies are linked to learning styles, personality, gender, and culture. O'Malley and Chamot [13] and Oxford [14] presented two detailed taxonomies of language learning strategies. The first taxonomy contained two major sets of strategies, cognitive (e.g., planning, organizing, evaluating), and a smaller third set, socio-affective (e.g., asking questions for clarification or verification). Oxford's taxonomy included cognitive, meta-cognitive, memory-related, social, affective, and compensatory strategies.

Relationships between strategy use and language proficiency were initially examined though the "good language learner" investigations, which resulted in general profiles of successful language learners and identified specific patterns of strategy use as success markers. In the 1990s, strategy use was linked to language proficiency in more than thirty studies around the world [13; 14], but the relationships were sometimes highly complex. Effects of learning strategy instruction on both proficiency and self-efficacy are the focus of much research [13].

Many demographic factors affect language learning. Two of these, gender and age, stand out as particularly important and are addressed here. But others that are not addressed include home language, foreign travel experience, and length of time in the country where the target language is used.

Gender makes a difference at all reference, hence females are superior in verbal skills, while males are superior in spatial skills, and these findings relate to brain functioning In general, females are slightly more feeling oriented, while males are slightly more thinking oriented [12]. Females enjoy cooperative and social learning, while males prefer individual, independent learning. Females use language learning strategies more frequently than males [8, 14, 16].

Age is also significant. Singleton and Lengyel [17] attack the critical period hypothesis, which suggests that learning a language at an early age is sufficient or necessary to attain native-like proficiency and that there is an age beyond which learning another language is not fully possible. Although younger learners do have some advantages (fluency and distinction of pronunciation), older learners have other advantages (syntax and morphology). However, many adults and children can become proficient in the target language under the right conditions, perhaps by following different routes.

To conclude the article up it should be noted that it's necessary to know more about our diverse students: their cultures, motivations, styles, strategies, anxiety, and other factors. This information can enable educators to develop new instructional techniques, curricula, and lesson plans to accommodate these varia-

tions. Language instruction should begin with an understanding of the ways students learn, and language researches should continue to promote this understanding

through well-designed studies.

Bibliography list

1. Bailey K., Nunan D. (eds.) Voices from the language classroom. N. Y.: Cambridge University press, 1996.

2. Banks J. A. Ethnicity, class, cognitive styles and motivational styles: Research and Teaching implications // Journal of Negro Education. 1991. Vol. 57.

3. Barcelos A. Relationships between Brazilian students' beliefs and their teachers' beliefs in an intensive ESL institute: Ph. D. diss. University of Alabama. Tuscaloosa, 2000.

4. Clément R., Dornyei Z., Noels K. A. Motivation, self-confidence, and group-cohesion in the foreign language classroom // Language Learning. 1994. Vol. 44.

5. Crookes G., Schmidt R. W. Motivation: Reopening the research agenda // Language Learning. 1991. Vol. 41.

6. Dreyer C. Teacher — student styles wars in South Africa: The silent battle // System.

1998. Vol. 26.

7. Ehrman M. Understanding second language learning difficulties. Thousand Oaks (CA): Sage, 1996.

8. Green J. M., Oxford R. L A closer look at learning strategies, L2 proficiency, and gender // TESOL Quarterly. 1995. Vol. 29.

9. Green S. Principles of Biopsychology. Hove (UK): Erlbaum, 1994.

10. HollidayA. Small Cultures // Applied Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press,

1999. Vol. 20.

11. Kramsch C. Language and Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998.

12. Myers I. B., MacCauley M. Manual: A guide to the developmental and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto (CA): Counseling Psychologist Press, 1985.

13. O'MalleyJ., ChamotA. Learning Strategies in second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990.

14. Oxford R. L. Language Learning Strategies: Cross Cultural Perspectives. Honolulu: University Press of Hawai'i, 1996.

15. Reid J. M. (ed.) Understanding learning styles in the second language classroom. Upper Saddle Rive (NJ): Prentice Hall, 1998.

16. Saleh A. The nexus of brain hemisphericity, personality types, temperaments, learning styles, learning strategies, gender, majors, and culture: Ph. D. diss. University of Alabama. Tuscaloosa, 1997.

17. Singleton D., Legyel Z. The age factor in second language acquisition: A critical look at the critical period hypothesis. Avon (UK): Multilingual Matters, 1995.

18. Tremblay P. F., Gardner R. C. Expending the motivation construct in language learning // The Modern Language Journal. 1995. Vol. 79.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.