Научная статья на тему 'Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary and Middle School Students '

Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary and Middle School Students Текст научной статьи по специальности «Науки об образовании»

CC BY
95
19
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Журнал
Psychology in Russia: State of the art
WOS
Scopus
ВАК
Область наук
Ключевые слова
self-efficacy / reliability / validity / academic achievement / metacognition / self-regulated learning

Аннотация научной статьи по наукам об образовании, автор научной работы — Diana R. Akhmedjanova

Background. Self-efficacy refers to students’ perceived confidence in their ability to tackle learning tasks. Research shows that self-efficacy serves as an important predictor of academic achievement and relates to students’ academic success, self-regulated learning, and motivation. It is therefore important to understand how self-efficacy develops and manifests itself in Russian schoolchildren and relates to their academic achievement. Objective. To establish evidence of the validity and reliability of domain-specific self-efficacy scales developed for elementary and middle school students. Design. Messick’s unified framework was used to establish validity. The surveys were administered to elementary and middle school students in two regions of Russia. Results. The pilot testing of the self-efficacy scales for elementary school, using exploratory (n = 972) and confirmatory (n = 972) factor analyses, resulted in a four-factor model, which was later confirmed with a different sample of elementary students (n = 1,392) with good reliability estimates (a = 0.75-0.82). The pilot testing of self-efficacy scales for middle school, using exploratory (n = 583) and confirmatory (n = 584) factor analyses, resulted in a three-factor model, showing excellent reliability estimates (a = 0.88-0.93). Conclusion. The evidence of construct validity suggests that the domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school students can be recommended for use by researchers and practitioners. The article presents ideas for additional validation studies and future research using domain-specific self-efficacy scales.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary and Middle School Students »

Psychology in Russia: State of the Art Volume 17, Issue 1, 2024

Russian Msri Psychological ill J Society

Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales

for Elementary and Middle School Students

Diana R. Akhmedjanova*

National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia ^Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Background. Self-efficacy refers to students' perceived confidence in their ability to tackle learning tasks. Research shows that self-efficacy serves as an important predictor of academic achievement and relates to students' academic success, self-regulated learning, and motivation. It is therefore important to understand how self-efficacy develops and manifests itself in Russian schoolchildren and relates to their academic achievement.

Objective. To establish evidence of the validity and reliability of domain-specific self-efficacy scales developed for elementary and middle school students.

Design. Messick's unified framework was used to establish validity. The surveys were administered to elementary and middle school students in two regions of Russia.

Results. The pilot testing of the self-efficacy scales for elementary school, using exploratory (n = 972) and confirmatory (n = 972) factor analyses, resulted in a four-factor model, which was later confirmed with a different sample of elementary students (n = 1,392) with good reliability estimates (a = 0.75-0.82). The pilot testing of self-efficacy scales for middle school, using exploratory (n = 583) and confirmatory (n = 584) factor analyses, resulted in a three-factor model, showing excellent reliability estimates (a = 0.88-0.93).

Conclusion. The evidence of construct validity suggests that the domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school students can be recommended for use by researchers and practitioners. The article presents ideas for additional validation studies and future research using domain-specific self-efficacy scales.

Keywords:

self-efficacy,

reliability,

validity,

academic

achievement,

metacognition,

self-regulated

learning

© Akhmedjanova, D.R., 2024

ISSN 2074-6857 (Print) / ISSN 2307-2202 (Online) http://psychologyinrussia.com

Introduction

Self-efficacy refers to students' perceived confidence in their ability to successfully tackle a task (Anderman & Wolters, 2008; Bandura, 1994, 2006). It relates to students' engagement with tasks and the types of strategies they use (Bandura, 1994; Pajares, 2002), as well as to their learning, motivation, achievement, and self-regulated learning (Bernacki et al., 2015; Cespedes et al., 2021; DiBenedetto & Schunk, 2022; Peura et al., 2019; Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2016; Talsma et al., 2018; Wood et al., 2022). Students with high self-efficacy tend to study hard, have high motivation and academic achievement, seek new opportunities to learn, regulate their own learning, interpret their academic failures due to a lack of sufficient effort, and perceive learning difficulties as challenges to overcome (Bandura, 1994, 2006; DiBenedetto & Schunk, 2022; Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2016). In contrast, students with low self-efficacy perceive their academic struggles as the result of low cognitive ability, avoid challenging tasks, and have low confidence in their capabilities to study well (Bandura, 1994, 2006).

Research has shown that children's self-efficacy changes with age. That is, younger children tend to overestimate their capabilities and show higher self-efficacy; however, with age and cognitive development, children's assessment of their ability to perform tasks improves. There is a general trend that as students get older and transition through school, their self-efficacy decreases (Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2016) and becomes more stable (Talsma et al., 2018). Self-efficacy is dynamic, and it changes depending on tasks, experiences of mastery, and successes or failures (DiBenedetto & Schunk, 2022). High self-efficacy tends to be a strong predictor of students' achievement and success (Bernacki et al., 2015; Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2016; Talsma et al., 2018).

Theoretical Framework

Historically, self-efficacy has been examined through the lens of social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1996) and described as a mutual interaction of personal, behavioral, and environmental factors (DiBenedetto & Schunk, 2022). In this study, we continue this tradition and situate self-efficacy within the Model of Self- and Socially Regulated Learning (Akhmedjanova, 2024; Figure 1), by recognizing the importance of personal, behavioral, and contextual factors. The model in Figure 1 is divided into three main sections: self-regulated learning (C-I, L-N), socially regulated learning (A-B, J-L), and culture (O). Instructional techniques (A-B) and formative assessment procedures (J-L) are examples of socially regulated learning (SoRL). Self-regulated learning (SRL) focuses on students' background knowledge and motivational beliefs, including self-efficacy, which lead to their decision on which strategies to use to complete tasks (C-I, M-N). Finally, culture (O) places both SRL and SoRL in a sociocultural setting.

The model includes multiple processes that are activated when students work on their tasks. As part of instruction (A), a teacher sets a learning task (B), which activates students' prior knowledge, knowledge of learning strategies, and motivational beliefs (C). For instance, if the task entails reading a chapter on quantum mechanics, students activate their prior knowledge of quantum mechanics, their interest in

reading about this topic, the value (high vs. low) they place on it, and their level of self-efficacy in understanding this text. Students interpret tasks (D) in idiosyncratic ways, and their interpretations influence their personal goals and task management (F), as well as their self-efficacy and motivation (C). Based on their task interpretation, students set mastery or performance goals (E). A mastery goal for reading about quantum mechanics could be to apply new knowledge while conducting an experiment in the physics lab, which would suggest some positive prior experiences with the topic and high self-efficacy. Alternatively, a performance goal could be to pass the final physics test. Depending on their goals, students manage the completion of the task (F) by applying different strategies (G). For example, while reading about quantum mechanics, students can select a quiet place and specific time slots in which to read. They can choose to watch YouTube videos or use other online resources to understand complex parts of the text. While doing the task, students tend to monitor their progress on the task (H) and adjust the strategies they are using. That is, while reading the chapter on quantum mechanics, students might discover that they still do not understand some parts and can ask their teacher for clarification. As a result of the processes outlined in C-G, students develop internal learning outcomes (I) or, for instance, a better understanding of quantum mechanics.

Later, the internal outcomes are manifested in externally observable outcomes (J), such as students' performance on tests or in lab experiments, which are assessed by teachers, peers, or technology (K). Feedback (K) provided by other sources identifies strengths and areas in need of improvement on the task, which contributes not only to the learning outcomes but also to students' self-efficacy. Talsma and colleagues (2018) showed a reciprocal relationship between students' performance and self-efficacy in their meta-analysis of 11 studies. That is, prior performance on a task relates to students' self-efficacy, just as students' self-efficacy relates to their future performance on similar tasks. Depending on students' interpretations of the feedback (L), their levels of self-efficacy can become higher or lower in comparison with their self-efficacy at the task interpretation stage (D). Students can revise their task (M) after receiving feedback and overall reflection (N) on the process of learning, which can also contribute to changes in their self-efficacy (C), as evidenced in a study of 9th-grade students studying algebra (Bernacki et al., 2015). All the processes outlined in Figure 1 are situated within a complex context that brings together students and teachers with various cultural backgrounds1.

Self-efficacy plays an important role in the processes outlined in Figure 1, because if students feel self-efficacious, they are more motivated to do the task. Also, if they

Culture (O) is one of the constructs represented in the theoretical framework. It is a very complex construct that requires extensive research and the development of the measurement tools to capture its role in self- and socially regulated learning. Currently, there is some research evidence suggesting differences in self-efficacy beliefs between individualistic cultures (Western) and collec-tivist cultures (Eastern European, Asian). It seems that students from Eastern European and Asian countries have lower self-efficacy than students from Western countries (Klassen, 2004). Results for Russian children are inconclusive, because they showed higher self-efficacy than children from East Berlin (Klassen, 2004; Oettingen, 1995); however, verbal and social persuasion and high SES contributed to academic self-efficacy for Russian-speaking immigrant children in Germany, who come from former Soviet countries (Gebauer et al., 2021). These findings contribute to the need for cross-cultural self-efficacy studies between children from Russia and other countries.

Figure 1. Model of self- and socially regulated learning

run into difficulties, these students are more likely to persist and try new strategies to complete the task and try similar ones in the future. As a result, it is important for teachers to know about students' levels of self-efficacy in order to adjust instructional practices. To do so, both practitioners and researchers need psychometrically sound instruments to measure self-efficacy.

Numerous self-efficacy scales exist, such as the Children's Self-Efficacy Scale (Bandura, 2006), the Children's Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale (Jinks & Morgan, 1999), and the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich et al., 1993), which includes self-efficacy as part of the inventory. Hence, domain-specific self-efficacy scales have been extensively examined and have shown moderate to high relations of self-efficacy to achievement in those domains and to other psychological constructs such as school engagement or well-being. For instance, researchers measured self-efficacy for narrative writing (Grenner et al., 2021), self-efficacy for programming and its relationship to computational thinking (Wei et al., 2021), math self-efficacy mediated the path between teacher autonomy support and student engagement (Li et al., 2020), and general self-efficacy mediated the relationship between academic self-concept and subjective well-being in adolescents in Chile (Cespedes et al., 2021).

Self-efficacy is well-researched in Russia in the field of psychology among university students (Berman, 2020; Kotova et al., 2021). There are also a few studies examining the self-efficacy of Russian adolescents (Gordeeva & Shepeleva, 2006; Gorlova, 2020; Shepeleva, 2008). For example, Shepeleva (2008) found that 10th- and 11th-grade students (n = 156) with higher academic self-efficacy were more likely to use such coping strategies as active engagement. In addition, adolescents' self-efficacy was related to their academic achievement. A more recent study of more than

15,000 Russian adolescents from PISA-2018 found that general self-efficacy mediated the relationship between reading and subjective well-being (Gorlova, 2020). The studies cited above focused on the academic and general self-efficacy of adolescent schoolchildren. However, Bandura (1994, 2006) stated that self-efficacy is dependent on the domain, and the level of specificity contributes to how students assess their confidence in performing certain tasks. In support of this position, Talsma et al. (2018) found that the effect sizes for relations between specific self-efficacy scales and academic performance are larger than for general self-efficacy scales. Therefore, in this study, we are proposing domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school students.

Current Study

The primary goal of this study is to examine the psychometric properties of the self-efficacy scales developed for elementary and middle school students (American Educational Research Association [AERA] et al., 2014). The unified validity framework was used to establish the evidence of the construct validity of self-efficacy scales (Kane, 2006; Messick, 1995). The standards for educational and psychological testing outline five sources of validity evidence such as content, response processes, internal structure, relations with other variables, and consequences of testing, including the reliability of the scale (AERA et al., 2014; Messick, 1995).

To establish evidence of validity and reliability, we posed the following research questions:

1. What is the evidence of validity based on the content of the self-efficacy scales?

2. What is the evidence of validity based on the internal structure of the self-efficacy scales?

3. What is the evidence of validity based on the relations of the self-efficacy scales with other variables?

4. What is the evidence of reliability of the self-efficacy scales?

This paper attempts to provide evidence of construct validity by identifying: (1) content representation by describing the development of the scales; (2) evidence of internal structure by examining factor structure; (3) relations with other variables by examining convergent and discriminant evidence; and (4) reliability by examining the internal consistency of each scale. These sources of validity evidence are described in the following sections.

Evidence Based on Content

Johnson and Morgan (2016) suggest developing new instruments in three successive phases: (1) operationalization of the construct; (2) pilot testing and scale development; and (3) survey model confirmation. Each phase is described in this paper.

Operationalization of Self-Efficacy

A group of experts in human development, self-regulated learning, and psychomet-rics examined available children's self-efficacy scales (Bandura, 2006; Jinks & Mor-

gan, 1999) to identify subscales and possible questionnaire items. However, none of the published instruments fit the Russian context for elementary school students. Therefore, we used Bandura's guidelines (2006) to develop domain-specific self-efficacy scales. Bandura (2006) maintained that researchers should target "activity domains and assess the multifaceted ways in which efficacy beliefs operate within the selected activity domain" (p. 310). Given the evidence that specific self-efficacy scales relate better to academic performance (Talsma et al., 2018), we treated self-efficacy as domain-specific and developed separate self-efficacy scales by school subjects.

Neuropsychological evidence suggests that abstract thinking skills are still developing in elementary school children (Uytun, 2018). Therefore, we chose to phrase self-efficacy items in terms of whether students can or cannot do certain tasks within a domain. We consulted the federal state educational standards to identify core competencies that students should develop within each domain by the end of elementary school to align self-efficacy items with the competencies outlined in the standards. As a result, six self-efficacy scales were developed: math (4 items), writing (4), grammar (4), speaking (4), reading (4), and natural studies (5). A similar procedure was applied to the development of scales for foreign language (5 items), biology (4), and physics (5) for middle school students. The response scale for both elementary and middle school scales ranged from 1 (I cannot do it at all) to 4 (I can do it well) to facilitate a better understanding by students, even though the unipolar scales, ranging from 0 to 100, show better psychometric properties (Bandura, 2006; Talsma et al., 2018).

Before pilot testing, the initial cognitive laboratory was conducted with two fourth-grade students resembling the demographic characteristics of the target population, to check for the readability and students' understanding of items on self-efficacy scales. Feedback from these students allowed us to revise some of the items to make them more age-appropriate and clear. Next, we carried out the pilot testing and survey model confirmation studies. The results of the pilot testing and survey model confirmation of self-efficacy scales for elementary school students are reported in the results section for Study 1.

The self-efficacy scales for middle school students (foreign language, biology, and physics) were pilot tested with another population in a different region of Russia. The results are reported in the results section for Study 2.

Methods

This study is part of a longitudinal project, using a mixed-methods design to examine factors related to the academic failure of schoolchildren in Russia (https://ioe.hse.ru/ failure-factors/). For the purposes of the present study, we used student data from the first wave collected in the fall of 2022 from fourth-grade students. Additionally, data collected for the research project implemented as part of the Basic Research Program at the National Research University Higher School of Economics (HSE University) were also used. Data were collected from students from public schools from the fourth through ninth grades in spring 2023.

Participants

Three separate samples were used to conduct various analyses, depending on the instruments used in elementary and middle schools. To examine the instruments for elementary school, the sample from the longitudinal study included 1,944 fourth graders (49.74% girls) from a metropolitan city (n = 1,242), small towns (n = 554), and rural areas (n = 148). Another sample of 1,392 responses from elementary school students (50.14% girls), collected in spring 2023, was used for the confirmatory analyses. The analyses were performed on the data from students in the fourth (n = 406), fifth (n = 482), and sixth (n = 504) grades. To examine the instruments for middle school, the sample included 1,167 students (55.3% girls, n = 645) from seventh (n = 345), eighth (n = 514), and ninth (n = 308) grades.

Instruments

In addition to the self-efficacy surveys evaluated in this study, the Self-Regulated Learning [SRL] Strategies Survey for Elementary School Students (Akhmedjanova & Lizunova, in press) and the metacognition scale (Lui et al., 2018) were used to check for relationships with other variables.

The SRL strategies survey is a 12-item scale focusing on strategies for managing environment, time, and learning, using a Likert-type scale (4 — almost always, 1 — almost never). Example item: "Iplan when I am going to do my homework." The internal consistency of the whole SRL scale is good, a = .83; wh = .71; wt = .85.

The metacognition scale is an adaptation of the SRL survey for the Diagnostic Assessment and Achievement of College Skills (DAACS, Lui et al., 2018). The survey includes the subscales of planning (5 items)2, monitoring (6 items)3, and reflection (3 items)4, using a Likert-type scale (4 — almost always, 1 — almost never). The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on Russian adolescents (n = 1167) confirmed the three-factor structure and indicated an excellent model fit, x2 (74) = 447.01, p < .000, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .07, SRMR = .04. The reliability estimates were good, a = .92; wh = .79; wt = .93.

Procedure

After receiving approval from the HSE University's Ethics Committee (#19), the data collection took place online in public schools in two regions of central Russia. Parents were informed about the purpose of the study and signed online consent forms, and children provided their assent to participate in this study.

Data Analyses

The data analysis was conducted in R Studio. The missing data analyses were done using the mice package (van Buuren et al., 2022). The psychometric package (Fletcher, 2022) was used for the exploratory factor analysis (EFA), lavaan (Rosseel et al.,

2 Example item; "I think about what I really need to learn before I begin a task"

3 Example item; "I ask myself periodically if I am meeting my goals."

4 Example item; "I ask myself if I learned as much as I could have once I finish a task."

2023) for the CFA, and the psych package (Revelle, 2022) to run Pearson r correlation analyses and identify Cronbach's alpha and McDonald's omega reliability estimates.

Missing Data

Missing data analyses were conducted for the fourth graders in the longitudinal study, revealing 0% missing data for students' gender and location to 14% for self-efficacy in natural studies. The Pearson's chi-squared test generated large p-values, which suggested that there was no association between missingness on the items for the self-regulation survey, self-efficacy for math, writing, grammar, reading, speaking, and natural studies, and the student's gender. Additionally, the results indicated that the missingness mechanism was not systematic, and missing values were possibly missing completely at random (MCAR). Therefore, it was decided to use listwise deletion, which resulted in deleting 717 cases with missing values and reducing the sample size to 1,944 observations, which sufficed for further analyses.

A similar analysis was not performed on the sample of elementary school students in the second dataset (Study 2) because there was no missing data. However, the missing data analyses were conducted on the sample of 1,469 responses from students in grades 7 through 9. The analyses revealed various degrees of missing data depending on the variable, ranging from 0% for students' gender and grade to 20% for the variable of self-efficacy for a foreign language. The Pearson's chi-squared test generated large p-values, which suggested that there was no association between missingness on the items of the metacognition survey, self-efficacy for foreign language, biology, and physics, and the student's gender and grade. Additionally, the results indicated that the missingness mechanism was not systematic, and missing values may have been missing completely at random. Therefore, it was decided to use listwise deletion, which resulted in deleting 302 cases with missing values and reducing the sample size to 1,167 observations that were used for analyses.

Since separate self-efficacy scales were developed for various school levels, the results are reported for elementary school in Study 1 and for middle school in Study 2.

Results

Study 1: Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary School

The pilot testing phase was conducted on the data of the fourth-grade students. To establish validity evidence based on the internal structure, we conducted exploratory and confirmatory analyses.

Exploratory Factor Analysis

Self-efficacy scales by domain were developed for the purposes of the longitudinal project; therefore, both EFA and CFA were used to identify the factor structure. The sample from the longitudinal study (n = 1,944) was randomly split into two equal parts, which were used for EFA (n = 972) and CFA (n = 972). The EFA was conducted on the original six self-efficacy scales for math, writing, grammar, reading, speaking,

and natural studies. The CFA allowed for verification of the factor structure proposed by the EFA.

Before conducting the EFA, the correlations and assumptions of factorability and sphericity were checked. The inter-item correlations indicated small to medium positive correlations among items (.14-.59). As expected, items within the same domains were more highly correlated with each other than with items from other domains. The Kaiser Meyer Olkin (KMO) factor adequacy overall estimate was .95, and the estimates for each item ranged from .91 to .97. The estimates of the Bartlett test of sphericity also suggested that a factor analysis was appropriate for this dataset, X2 (300) = 3,560.48, p < .001.

The factor structure based on eigenvalues suggested a five-factor model; scree plots of the parallel analysis suggested a seven-factor model. Since the scale development included six distinct domains, the six-factor model was also checked. All models indicated a good model fit (Table 1); however, the factors for grammar and speaking did not work as expected. For example, in a five-factor model, the items for reading and speaking were loaded on one factor. Similarly, only two grammar items had loadings above 0.30 on a separate factor. As a result, it was decided to run a four-factor model. The results indicated a good model fit; therefore, it was decided to leave out the scales of grammar, speaking, and item 5 from the scale of self-efficacy for natural studies due to its low factor loading estimate.

Table 1

EFA Model Fit Indices (n = 972)

Model x2 p TLI RMSEA RMSR

7-factor model 313.12 <.0001 .96 .03 .02

6-factor model 413.7 < .0001 .95 .04 .02

5-factor model 520.84 <.0001 .94 .04 .02

4-factor model 689.2 <.0001 .92 .05 .03

Note. TLI — Tucker Lewis Index, RMSEA — Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, RMSR — Root Mean Square Residual

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The CFA analysis was conducted on the second half of the sample (n = 972) to examine the four-factor structure. The diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) estimator was used to estimate the model parameters due to the ordinal nature of the self-efficacy scales. The CFA indicated an excellent model fit, x2(98) = 292.02, p < .000, CFI = . 99, TLI = . 99, RMSEA = . 04, SRMR = . 04, with all items having medium to large factor loadings (.67-.89). The x2/df coefficient resulted in an estimate of 2.97. X reports on the item-level statistics and Appendix A includes the self-efficacy items for elementary school. The survey confirmation study was conducted on a sample of the fourth, fifth, and sixth graders in spring 2023, using the scales of self-efficacy in math, writing, and reading. CFA revealed an excellent model fit, x2 (51) = 264.09,

p < .000, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .05, SRMR = .04, with all items showing medium to large factor loadings. The x2/df coefficient resulted in 5.17.

Table 2

Reliability Indices and Item Levels Estimates after CFA for Self-Efficacy Scales (n = 972)

St. alpha Alpha if item is dropped Omega Omega hierarchical Total Mean (SD) Item total correlation Item total if item is dropped

SE for math .8 .74 .83

Item 1 .74 3.2 (.76) .70 .61

Item 2 .71 2.9 (.82) .76 .66

Item 3 .75 3.1 (.87) .66 .59

Item 4 .77 2.9 (.95) .61 .55

SE for writing .75 .68 .8

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Item 5 .69 2.7 (.81) .64 .55

Item 6 .70 3.0 (.80) .62 .53

Item 7 .67 2.7 (.83) .68 .59

Item 8 .71 2.9 (.83) .60 .52

SE for reading .79 .79 .81

Item 9 .75 2.7 (.85) .66 .58

Item 10 .72 2.9 (.80) .73 .64

Item 11 .71 2.9 (.75) .75 .66

Item 12 .78 2.5 (.80) .57 .51

SE for natural studies .82 .74 .86

Item 13 .79 2.8 (.80) .78 .66

Item 14 .77 2.8 (.81) .79 .62

Item 15 .75 3.3 (.75) .82 .66

Item 16 .76 3.4 (.71) .74 .65

Note: SE — self-efficacy

Evidence Based on Relations to Other Variables

In this project, we did not measure self-efficacy using other self-efficacy scales. However, since each subscale is domain-specific but measures students' self-efficacy, each subscale can be used as convergent evidence of validity. Discriminant evidence of validity was established using the SRL survey for elementary school students.

The correlations of the self-regulated learning strategies resulted in significant positive low correlations with the domain-specific self-efficacy scales, ranging from

0.21 to 0.37 (Table 3), which suggests that these scales measure different yet positively related constructs. The correlation estimates among domain-specific self-efficacy scales are moderate and significant. This suggests that they measure a similar trait, which contributes to the convergent evidence of validity. The correlations between self-efficacy in math and students' math results (0.38) and self-efficacy in reading and students' reading results (0.18) are positive and significant, which suggests that as students gain higher academic results, their self-efficacy increases5.

Reliability

The reliability analysis was performed by estimating both Cronbach's alpha and McDonald's omega, which provide complementary and robust evidence of internal consistency (Deng & Chan, 2017). The reliability indices for each self-efficacy scale are good (Table 2), suggesting that the scales measure self-efficacy within their respective domains.

Table 3

Correlations Among Subscales of SRL Survey and Self-Efficacy Scales (n = 1,671)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

SRL 1

SE math .21*** 1

SE writing .37*** .51*** 1

SE reading .32*** .57*** .62*** 1

SE nature .31*** .51*** .55*** .61*** 1

Math -.04 .36*** .13*** .19*** .15*** 1

Reading -.02 .26*** .17*** .18*** .17*** .46*** 1

Mean 2.75 3.03 2.82 2.76 3.09 51.95 52.02

(SD) (0.59) (0.66) (0.62) (0.63) (0.60) (9.82) (9.01)

Note: ***p < .0001; **p < .001; *p < .05; SE — self-efficacy

Study 2: Self-Efficacy Scales for Middle School

As in Study 1, the internal structure of the surveys was examined using the EFA and CFA. The sample (n = 1,167) was randomly split into equal parts for EFA (n = 583) and CFA (n = 584). First, the EFA is described, followed by the CFA.

Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses

Before conducting the EFA, data correlations and assumptions of factorability and sphericity were checked. The inter-item correlations indicated small to medium positive correlations among items (.09-.78). The KMO factor adequacy overall estimate

Additional results on differences between boys and girls in different grades are provided in Appendix C.

was 0.9, and the Bartlett test of sphericity was, x2 (91) = 5810.75, p < .001, suggesting that it was appropriate to conduct a factor analysis.

The factor structure based on eigenvalues and the scree plots of the parallel analysis suggested a three-factor model, which corresponded with the three domains of foreign language, biology, and physics. The three-factor model indicated a good model fit, x2 (52) = 165.73, p < .000, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .06, SRMR = .02.

The CFA was conducted on the second half of a sample (n = 584) to examine the three-factor structure of self-efficacy for foreign language, biology, and physics. The CFA indicated an excellent model fit, x2 (74) = 126.56, p < .000, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .03, SRMR=0.03. The x2/df coefficient resulted in an estimate of 1.71. In addition, all items had medium to large factor loadings. Table 4 reports on the itemlevel statistics and Appendix B includes the self-efficacy items for middle school.

Table 4

Reliability Indices and Item-Level Estimates after CFA (n = 584)

St. Alpha if Omega Omega Mean Item total Item total alpha item is hierarchical Total (SD) correlation if item is dropped dropped

SE for foreign .93 .92 .94

language

Item 1 .91 2.6 (.92) .86 .83

Item 2 .91 2.4 (.96) .88 .84

Item 3 .92 2.2 (.96) .83 .80

Item 4 .92 2.6 (.91) .82 .79

Item 5 .91 2.4 (.91) .84 .81

SE for biology .88 .85 .9

Item 6 .85 2.5 (.80) .81 .75

Item 7 .84 2.8 (.78) .83 .77

Item 8 .85 2.7 (.76) .79 .74

Item 9 .86 2.5 (.84) .75 .70

SE for physics .93 .9 .95

Item 10 .92 2.5 (.87) .83 .80

Item 11 .91 2.5 (.84) .88 .84

Item 12 .91 2.5 (.83) .87 .84

Item 13 .92 2.4 (.83) .84 .81

Item 14 .92 2.4 (.89) .80 .80

Note: SE — self-efficacy

Evidence Based on Relations with Other Variables

The initial convergent evidence was established by examining the correlations between domains of self-efficacy scales. Discriminant evidence of validity was examined using the metacognitive survey.

The correlations of all subscales with each other resulted in significant positive low estimates, ranging from .22 to .45 (Table 5), which provides convergent and discriminant evidence of validity. The correlation estimates among domain-specific self-efficacy scales are low, positive, and significant (.30-.45). The subscales of planning, monitoring, and reflection indicated positive, low yet significant correlations with self-efficacy for foreign language, biology, and physics (.21-.26). Low correlations contribute to the discriminant evidence of validity. Hence, each self-efficacy subscale resulted in positive, significant yet low correlations with the corresponding subject domains6. The results provide initial convergent and discriminant evidence of validity for self-efficacy scales in middle school.

Reliability

Reliability was estimated using Cronbach's alpha and McDonald's omega. The reliability indices for each self-efficacy scale were high (a = .88-.93; = .85-.92; wt = .9.95; Table 4), suggesting that the scales measure self-efficacy within their respective domains.

Table 5

Correlations Among Subscales of Metacognition Survey and Self-Efficacy Subscales (n = 1,167)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

Planning 1

Monitoring .67*** 1

Reflection .62*** .75*** 1

SE FL .22*** .22*** .22*** 1

SE biology .25*** .21*** .26*** .30*** 1

SE physics .25*** .25*** .23*** .36*** .45*** 1

FL .13*** .13*** .08** .41*** .13*** .27*** 1

Biology .17*** .16*** .13*** .21*** .34*** .28*** .53*** 1

Physics .13*** .12*** .07** .25*** .18*** .44*** .45*** .45*** 1

Mean 2.75 2.79 2.68 2.45 2.62 2.47 4.01 4.07 3.71

(SD) (.63) (.66) (.74) (.81) (.66) (.75) (.77) (.7) (.86)

Note: ***p < .0001; SE — self-efficacy; FL —foreign language

Discussion

The goal of this study was to establish evidence of the construct validity of domain-specific self-efficacy scales developed for elementary and middle school students.

6 Additional results on differences between boys and girls in different grades are provided in Appendix D.

The exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses results of self-efficacy scales for elementary school indicated that four out of six initial scales — self-efficacy in math, writing, reading, and natural studies — showed appropriate psychometric properties and a four-factor structure. The exploratory and confirmatory analyses of self-efficacy scales for middle school students also provided good evidence of construct validity by suggesting three distinct factors of self-efficacy for foreign language, biology, and physics. As a result, self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school represent domain specificity, as suggested by Albert Bandura (2006).

Initial convergent evidence of validity was examined using correlations between self-efficacy domains in elementary and middle school samples. The results for the elementary school students indicated medium and significant correlations, suggesting that domain-specific self-efficacy scales are measuring a close construct. This was especially evident for self-efficacy in reading, which had medium correlations with all other domains ranging from .56 to .63. Conceptually, medium correlations between self-efficacy in reading and self-efficacy in other domains are understandable, because if students have good reading skills and high self-efficacy in reading, then they can read and, hopefully, understand what they are expected to do in other school subjects (Chen et al., 2021). In the middle school sample, the correlations between self-efficacy scales were significant yet low; the only correlation approaching medium estimates was between self-efficacy in biology and physics (.45), suggesting positive relationships between life and hard sciences. It can be concluded that convergent evidence of validity in this case is weak, because it should be established using another instrument measuring self-efficacy (AERA et al., 2014).

Correlations between self-efficacy scales and the SRL survey resulted in significant yet low correlations, which suggests that the two surveys measure related but distinct constructs for elementary school students. A similar pattern was observed for correlations between self-efficacy scales for middle school and subscales of the metacognitive survey: planning, monitoring, and reflection. These results contribute to the discriminant evidence of validity.

Finally, domain-specific self-efficacy scales were related to students' academic performance in their respective domains, both in elementary and middle school samples. However, these relationships were low and the only correlation between self-efficacy in physics and students' grades in physics approached a medium estimate (0.44). These results align with previous research studies reporting low to medium correlations between self-report measures of self-efficacy and academic achievement (DiBenedetto & Schunk, 2022).

Reliability analyses resulted in good estimates for each self-efficacy scale for elementary school as measured by Cronbach's alphas and McDonald's omegas. Similar analyses for middle school scales resulted in good estimates for self-efficacy for foreign language and biology, and excellent estimates for self-efficacy in physics. This evidence contributes to the internal consistency of each domain-specific self-efficacy scale for elementary and middle school.

Conclusion

The evidence of construct validity suggests that the domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school students can be recommended for use both by researchers and practitioners. The scientific contribution of this paper is that it proposes domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary and middle school students, which have been developed using evidence-based guidelines in the fields of education and psychology, ensuring alignment with the federal state educational standards. As a result, the self-efficacy scales align with the requirements reflected in the legal documents for elementary and secondary education in Russia. Hence, these scales can facilitate future research studies in elementary and middle school settings to examine relationships between self-efficacy and academic achievement, as well as their contributions to student characteristics in line with studies in other countries (Grenner et al., 2021; Li et al., 2020).

In future research, scholars are invited to examine the role of self-efficacy in students' academic results, as in a study examining its relation to computational thinking (Wei et al., 2021). Our initial examination of correlations between self-efficacy scales and students' scores in subject domains resulted in positive yet low relations, which also requires further mediation and moderation analyses. We invite researchers to use sophisticated statistical techniques such as structural equation modeling (Kline, 2023) or cluster analyses (Wierzchon & Klopotek, 2018) to examine relationships of self-efficacy with other variables and across multiple groups. In our own research, we found that self-efficacy in math and reading mediates the relationship between self-regulated learning and academic achievement and moderates the relationship between subjective well-being and academic results (Kanonire et al., 2023) in a sample of elementary students. Another area of future research could focus on establishing additional robust convergent and discriminant evidence of validity for self-efficacy scales and collecting evidence of response processes and consequences of testing as part of construct validity (AERA et al., 2014).

Limitations

Even though the self-efficacy scales resulted in appropriate psychometric properties, this study has inherent limitations, which might affect the generalization of its results. To establish convergent evidence of validity, we relied on different domains of the same self-efficacy scales. While domain-specific self-efficacy scales for elementary school went through all phases of instrument development (Johnson & Morgan, 2016), scales for middle school were subject only to operationalization of self-efficacy (phase 1) and pilot testing (phase 2). It is recommended to collect additional data using self-efficacy scales for foreign language, biology, and physics to conduct model confirmation analyses (phase 3).

Ethics Statement

Ethics approval (#19) was received from the Ethics Committee at the National Research University Higher School of Economics prior to collecting data. All participants provided informed consent to participate in this study.

Informed Consent from the Participants' Legal Guardians (if the participants were minors)

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Parents were informed about the purpose of the study and signed online consent forms, and children provided their assent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

The author suggested the idea and actively participated in scale development and testing. The author prepared the article and performed all the calculations. However, the results were discussed with the team members of the longitudinal project examining the factors related to the academic failure of schoolchildren in Russia (https:// ioe.hse.ru/failure-factors/).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This article is an output of two research projects implemented as part of the Basic Research Program at the National Research University Higher School of Economics (HSE University). I would also like to express my gratitude to Katerina Polivanova, Alexandra Bochaver, and Roksana Bayramyan for their assistance during the scale development process.

References

Akhmedjanova, D.R. (2024). Rol' sotsial'noi reguliatsii obucheniia v shkol'nom vozraste [Role of social and self-regulated learning in school contexts]. Voprosy obrazovaniia [Educational Studies], 1, 11-43. https://doi.org/10.17323/vo-2024-17280 Akhmedjanova, D.R., & Lizunova, E. G. (in press). Development and initial validation of the self-regulated learning strategies survey for elementary school students. Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics.

American Educational Research Association [AERA], American Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education, Joint Committee on Standards for Educational, and Psychological Testing (US). (2014). Standards for educational and psychological testing. American Educational Research Association. Anderman, E. M., & Wolters, C. A. (2008). Goals, values, and affect: Influences on student motivation. In P. A. Alexander & P.H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 369-390). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-07986-017 Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-

efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 307-337). Information Age Publishing. Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V.S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior, Vol. 4

(pp. 71-81). Academic Press. Berman, N.D. (2020) The impact of self-efficacy on student learning. World of Science. Pedagogy and Psychology, 2. https://mir-nauki.com/PDF/83PDMN220.pdf

Bernacki, M. L., Nokes-Malach, T. J., & Aleven, V. (2015). Examining self-efficacy during learning: Variability and relations to behavior, performance, and learning. Metacognition and Learning, 10, 99-117. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9127-x Céspedes, C., Rubio, A., Viñas, F., Cerrato, S. M., Lara-Órdenes, E., & Ríos, J. (2021). Relationship between self-concept, self-efficacy, and subjective well-being of native and migrant adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.620782 Chen, J., Zhang, Y., & Hu, J. (2021). Synergistic effects of instruction and affect factors on high-and low-ability disparities in elementary students' reading literacy. Reading and Writing, 34, 199-230. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10070-0 Deng, L., & Chan, W. (2017). Testing the difference between reliability coefficients alpha and omega. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 77, 185-203. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164416658325 DiBenedetto, M. K., & Schunk, D. H. (2022). Assessing academic self-efficacy. In M.S. Khine & T. Nielsen

(Eds.), Academic self-efficacy in education: nature, assessment, and research (pp. 11-37). Springer. Fletcher, T. (2022). Package "psychometric" [R package]. Available at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/

packages/psychometric/psychometric.pdf (Accessed 1 December 2023). Gebauer, M.M., McElvany, N., Koller, O., & Schober, C. (2021). Cross-cultural differences in academic self-efficacy and its sources across socialization contexts. Social Psychology of Education, 24(6), 1407-1432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282914531707 Gordeeva, T.O., & Shepeleva E.A. (2006) Gender differences in academic and social self-efficacy and coping strategies in modern Russian adolescents. Moscow University Psychology Bulletin, 3, 7885.

Gorlova, N.V. (2020). Relationship of subjective well-being and academic self-efficacy of school children.

[Master's degree thesis, Higher School of Economics]. https://www.hse.ru/edu/vkr/370521196 Grenner, E., Johansson, V., van de Weijer, J., & Sahlén, B. (2021). Effects of intervention on self-efficacy and text quality in elementary school students' narrative writing. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocol-ogy, 46(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/14015439.2019.1709539 Jinks, J., & Morgan, V. (1999). Children's perceived academic self-efficacy: An inventory scale. The Clearing House, 72(4), 224-230. https://doi.org/10.1080/00098659909599398 Kane, M. (2006). Content-related validity evidence in test development. In S.M. Downing and T.M.

Haladyna (Eds.), Handbook of test development (pp. 131-153). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kanonire, T., Akhmedjanova, D., & Zakharov, A. (2023, July 5-8). The relationship of subjective well-being and self-efficacy with academic achievement of fourth grade children: A moderation effect [Paper presentation]. 44th Annual Conference of the International School Psychology Association, Italy. Klassen, R.M. (2004). Optimism and realism: A review of self-efficacy from a cross-cultural perspective.

International Journal of Psychology, 39(3), 205-230. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000330 Kline, R. B. (2023). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford publications. Kotova, S., Hasanova, I., Sadovnikova, N., Komarov, E., & Wenbin, L. (2021). Self-efficacy as a personality predictor of the career orientations of college students. International Journal of Instruction, 14(4), 1047-1064. https://doi.org/10.29333/iji.2021.14460a Li, W., Gao, W., & Sha, J. (2020). Perceived teacher autonomy support and school engagement of Tibetan students in elementary and middle schools: Mediating effect of self-efficacy and academic emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00050 Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: validation of inferences from persons' responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. American Psychologist, 50, 741-749. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.9.741 Oettingen, G. (1995). Cross-cultural perspectives on self-efficacy. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self- efficacy in

changing societies (pp. 149-176). Cambridge University Press. Pajares, F. (2002). Gender and perceived self-efficacy in self-regulated learning. Theory into Practice, 41(2), 116-125. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15430421tip4102_8 Peura, P., Aro, T., Viholainen, H., Raikkonen, E., Usher, E. L., Sorvo, R., & Aro, M. (2019). Reading self-efficacy and reading fluency development among primary school children: Does specificity of self-efficacy matter? Learning and Individual Differences, 73, 67-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. lindif.2019.05.007

Pintrich, P.R., Smith, D.A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W.J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801-813. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164493053003024 Revelle, W. (2023). Package "psych" [R package]. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/

packages/psych/psych.pdf (Accessed December 1, 2023). Rosseel, Y., Jorgensen, T. D., Rockwood, N., Oberski, D., Byrnes, J., Vanbrabant, L., ... & Jamil, H. (2023). Package "lavaan" [R package]. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lavaan/ lavaan.pdf (Accessed December 1, 2023). Schunk, D.H., & DiBenedetto, M.K. (2016). Self-efficacy theory in education. In K.R. Wentzel & D.B.

Miele (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 34-54). Routledge. Shepeleva, E.A. (2008). Osobennosti uchebnoi i sotsial'noi samoeffektivnosti shkol'nikov [Features of educational and social self-efficacy of schoolchildren]. [PhD Thesis, Lomonosov Moscow State University].

Talsma, K., Schuz, B., Schwarzer, R., & Norris, K. (2018). I believe, therefore I achieve (and vice versa): A meta-analytic cross-lagged panel analysis of self-efficacy and academic performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 136-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.015 Uytun, M.C. (2018). Development period of prefrontal cortex. In A. Starcevic and B. Filipovic (Eds.), Prefrontal cortex (pp. 3-22). IntechOpen. van Buuren, S., Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K., Vink, G., Schouten, R., Robitzsch, A., Rockenshaub, P., ... & Oberman, H. (2022). Package "mice" [R package]. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/ web/packages/mice/index.html (Accessed December 1, 2023). Wei, X., Lin, L., Meng, N., Tan, W., & Kong, S. C. (2021). The effectiveness of partial pair programming on elementary school students' computational thinking skills and self-efficacy. Computers & Education, 160, 104023. https://doi.org/10.1016/jxompedu.2020.104023 Wierzchon, S.T., & Klopotek, M.A. (2018). Modern algorithms of cluster analysis (Vol. 34). Springer International Publishing.

Wood, C., Tramontano, C., & Hemsley, S. (2022). Self-efficacy in the classroom: The roles of motivation, positivity and resilience. In M.S. Khine & T. Nielsen (Eds.), Academic self-efficacy in education: Nature, assessment, and research (pp. 11-37). Springer.

Original manuscript received January 24, 2024 Revised manuscript accepted March 10, 2024 First published online March 15, 2024

To cite this article: Akhmedjanova, D.R. (2024). Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary and Middle School Students, Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 17(1), 45-66. DOI: 10.11621/ pir.2024.0103

Appendix A

Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary School

Instructions: Подумай и ответь, можешь ли ты выполнить задания по разным предметам. Задания выполнять не нужно,

Поэтому отвечай честно, смог/смогла бы ты выполнить задания.

Шкала ответов: 1 — совсем не могу; 2 — могу НЕ очень хорошо; 3 — могу хорошо; 4 — могу очень хорошо.

Russian

English

СЭ математика

Можешь ли ты решить пример по математике? Можешь ли ты решить задачу по математике? Можешь ли ты посчитать площадь прямоугольника?

Можешь ли ты назвать единицы длины?

Self-efficacy for mathematics

Can you solve a math equation? Can you solve a math problem? Can you calculate the area of the rectangle?

Can you name the units of length?

СЭ русский язык — письмо

Можешь ли ты что-то написать без ошибок под диктовку учителя?

Можешь ли ты переписать без ошибок текст с доски или из книги в тетрадь?

Можешь ли ты написать изложение с составлением плана?

Можешь ли ты написать короткое сочинение по картинке или на заданную тему?

Self-efficacy for writing

Can you write without mistakes following the teacher's dictation?

Can you rewrite the text without mistakes from the board or textbook into your notebook?

Can you write a composition following an outline?

Can you write a short essay describing a picture or topic?

Литература — чтение

Можешь ли ты быстро (бегло) прочесть рассказ и понять при этом прочитанное? Можешь ли пересказать главную мысль прочитанного рассказа?

Можешь ли ты прочитать текст и ответить на вопросы учителя по этому тексту? Можешь ли ты понять значение новых слов в рассказе без помощи взрослого?

Self-efficacy for reading

Can you fluently read a story and understand its meaning?

Can you summarize the main idea of a story?

Can you read a story and answer the teacher's questions about it? Can you understand new words in a story without help from adults?

Окружающий мир

Можешь ли ты назвать основные группы животных и растений?

Можешь ли ты объяснить взаимосвязь между природой и человеком?

Можешь ли ты рассказать о правилах гигиены? Можешь ли ты рассказать о правилах безопасности дома и на улице?

Self-efficacy for natural studies

Can you name the main groups of animals and plants?

Can you explain the relationship between nature and humans?

Can you describe the rules of hygiene? Can you describe the safety rules at home and outside?

Appendix B

Domain-Specific Self-efficacy Scales for Middle School

Instructions: Подумай и ответь, можешь ли ты выполнить задания по разным предметам. Задания выполнять не нужно,

Поэтому отвечай честно, смог/смогла бы ты выполнить задания.

Шкала ответов: 1 — совсем не могу; 2 — могу НЕ очень хорошо; 3 — могу хорошо; 4 — могу очень хорошо.

Russian

English

СЭ иностранный язык

Можешь ли ты рассказать о себе или о событии на_языке?

Можешь ли ты поддержать разговор на_

языке с другими людьми (одноклассники, учителя, иностранцы)?

Можешь ли ты написать сочинение или историю на_языке?

Можешь ли ты прочитать текст на_языке

и понять его содержание без словаря? Можешь ли ты понять на слух разговор на _языке при просмотре видео?

Self-efficacy for foreign language

Can you talk about yourself or an event in the_language?

Can you speak with other people (peers, teachers, foreigners) in the_language?

Can you write an essay in the_language?

Can you read a story in the_language

and understand its meaning?

Can you understand conversations when

watching videos in the_language?

Self-efficacy for biology

Can you describe and classify various types of plants?

Can you describe and classify v of animals? Can you describe various processes happening in the human body? Can you apply scientific methods of observation, measurement, and experimenting to describe living beings?

СЭ биология

Можешь ли ты описать и классифицировать

разные виды растений?

Можешь ли ты описать и классифицировать

разные виды животных?

Можешь ли ты описать различные процессы,

происходящие в организме человека?

Можешь ли ты применять научные методы

наблюдения, измерения и эксперимента для

описания живых существ?

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

I various types

СЭ физика

Можешь ли ты проводить прямые измерения физических величин (расстояние, время, объем, температура и т.д.)?

Можешь ли ты проводить исследование физических величин и делать выводы по результатам исследования?

Можешь ли ты проводить опыты по наблюдению физических явлений или свойств тел? Можешь ли ты обосновывать выбор способа измерения или измерительного прибора при проведении исследований? Можешь ли ты распознавать проявление изученных физических явлений (кристаллизация, кипение, конденсация, взаимодействие магнитов и так далее) в окружающем мире?

Self-efficacy for physics

Can you apply physical measurements such as distance, time, volume, temperature, etc.?

Can you conduct experiments using physical measurements and draw conclusions based on the results?

Can you conduct experiments to observe physical phenomena or properties of bodies? Can you justify the choice of measurement methods and instruments when conducting research?

Can you recognize physical phenomena such as crystallization, boiling, condensation, interaction of magnets, etc. in the world around you?

Domain-Specific Self-Efficacy Scales for Elementary and Middle School Students 65 Appendix C Table 1

T-test Analysis of Fourth-Grade Students' Academic Performance and Self-Efficacy by Gender (n = 1,671)

Girls Boys

M SD M SD t(df) P Cohen's D

Math 51.41 9.95 52.51 9.66 -2.31 (1,668.3) .02 -.11

Reading 52.39 9.11 51.65 8.89 1.68 (1,668.6) .09 -

SE Math 2.95 .65 3.11 .66 -4.78 (1,666.8) <.0001 -.23

SE Reading 2.75 .64 2.79 .62 -1.32 (1,668.5) .18 -

Note: SE — self-efficacy

Table 2

T-test Analysis of Students' Academic Performance and Self-Efficacy by Gender in Grades 4-6 (n = 1,167)

Girls Boys

M SD M SD t(df) P Cohen's D

Math 3.96 .76 3.93 .78 .63 (1,389.2) .53 -

Russian 3.99 .72 3.82 .71 4.62 (1,389.9) <.0001 .25

Reading 4.35 .71 4.24 .71 2.93 (1,390) .003 .16

SE Math 2.87 .68 3.03 .67 -4.49 (1,389.8) <.0001 -.24

SE Russian 3.03 .59 2.81 .58 6.96 (1,389.9) <.0001 .37

SE Reading 2.76 .64 2.79 .62 -1.12 (1,388.3) .26 -

Note: SE — self-efficacy

Table 3

ANOVA Analysis of Students' Academic Performance and Self-Efficacy by Grade (n = 1,167)

4 5 6 ANOVA

M SD M SD P n2

Math 4.16 (.74) 3.97 (.76) 3.76 (.77) 31.52 <.0001 .04

Russian 4.08 (.69) 3.96 (.72) 3.72 (.71) 30.85 <.0001 .04

Reading 4.46 (.65) 4.35 (.72) 4.10 (.71) 32.3 <.0001 .02

SE Math 3.17 (.65) 2.89 (.68) 2.82 (.66) 34.8 <.0001 .05

SE Russian 2.91 (.63) 2.92 (.59) 2.93 (.57) .15 .86 -

SE Reading 2.91 (.65) 2.72 (.61) 2.74 (.62) 11.56 <.0001 .02

Note: SE — self-efficacy

66 Akhmedjanova, D.R. Appendix D

Table 1

T-test Analysis of Students' Academic Performance and Self-Efficacy by Gender in Grades 7-9 (n = 1,392)

Girls Boys

M SD M SD t(df) P Cohen's D

FL 4.16 .71 3.81 .79 7.96 (1,053.6) <.0001 .47

Biology 4.19 .63 3.92 .76 6.5 (1,007.6) <.0001 .39

Physics 3.81 .85 3.60 .87 4.18 (1,101.4) <.0001 .25

SE FL 2.45 .81 2.44 .81 .30 (1,115.6) .76 -

SE Biology 2.64 .64 2.59 .69 1.08 (1,075.8) .28 -

SE Physics 2.39 .74 2.55 .77 -3.72 (1,095.9) <.001 -.22

Note: SE — self-efficacy; FL — foreign language

Table 2

ANOVA Analysis of Students' Academic Performance and Self-Efficacy by Grade (n = 1,392)

7 8 9 ANOVA

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F (2, 1164) P n2

FL 4.03 (.72) 3.96 (.77) 4.05 (.81) 1.58 .21 -

Biology 4.16 (.65) 4.04 (.73) 4.04 (.71) 3.57 .03 .006

Physics 3.76 (1.15) 3.68 (.71) 3.73 (.74) 0.86 .42 -

SE FL 2.44 (.83) 2.36 (.78) 2.6 (.84) 8.49 <.0001 .01

SE Biology 2.61 (.64) 2.66 (.64) 2.55 (.72) 2.57 .08 -

SE Physics 2.54 (.73) 2.37 (.73) 2.55 (.81) 8.21 <.001 .01

Note: SE — self-efficacy; FL — foreign language

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.