Научная статья на тему 'Automated Research in Comparative Psychology: Limitations and New Directions'

Automated Research in Comparative Psychology: Limitations and New Directions Текст научной статьи по специальности «Физика»

CC BY
348
76
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Область наук
Ключевые слова
Behavior / behavioral research / automated techniques / behavioral scientists / automated equipment. / поведение / изучение поведения / автоматизированные методы / бихевиористы / автоматизированное оборудование.

Аннотация научной статьи по физике, автор научной работы — Christopher Varnon A., Harley Lang, Charles I. Abramson

Behavioral research is often enhanced by automated techniques, where experimental parameters and detection of behavior are controlled by electromechanical systems. Automated research promotes refinements in measurement, greater experimental control, longer durations of data collection, reduction of observer fatigue, and may permit new types of research to be conducted. In comparative psychology, use of automated techniques are often restricted to popular model organisms of fields such as behavior analysis and behavioral neuroscience. One factor contributing to this species-restriction may be the availability of automated research equipment, as most commercial research equipment is designed for rodents, and many researchers lack the skills required to create their own automated equipment. However, there are alternatives to commercial equipment, as some behavioral scientists have made available their own species-flexible, low-cost research equipment. In this paper, we provide three reviews. We first review recent trends in automated comparative psychology research, and then relate this to a second review on currently available automated research equipment. We also review affordable alternatives to commercial equipment that have been designed by behavioral scientists. Finally, we discuss useful technological skills that may allow comparative psychologists to take automation into their own hands and design equipment specific to their species and research topic.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Автоматизированные исследования в области сравнительной психологии: ограничения и новые направления

Исследование поведения часто проводится с помощью автоматизированных методов, где экспериментальные параметры и обнаружение поведения контролируются электромеханическими системами. Автоматизированное исследование способствует усовершенствованиям в измерении, большем экспериментальном контроле, обеспечивают более продолжительное хранение данных, уменьшение усталости наблюдателя, и могут применяться новые виды исследований. В сравнительной психологии использование автоматизированных методов часто ограничивается популярными моделями , такими как анализ поведения и поведенческая нейробиология. Одним из факторов, способствующих этому ограничению разновидностей, может быть доступность автоматизированного оборудования исследования, поскольку большая часть коммерческого оборудования исследования разработана для грызунов, и многие исследователи испытывают недостаток в навыках, требуемых для создания их собственного автоматизированного оборудования. Однако есть альтернативы коммерческому оборудованию, поскольку некоторые бихевиористы сделали доступным свое собственное гибкое к разновидностям, недорогостоящее оборудование для исследований. В данной статье мы представляем три отзыва. Мы сначала рассматриваем недавние тенденции в автоматизированном сравнительном исследовании психологии, и затем соотносим его со вторым анализом относительно доступности в настоящее время автоматизированного оборудования для исследований. Мы также рассматриваем доступные альтернативы коммерческому оборудованию, которые были разработаны бихевиористами. Наконец, мы обсуждаем полезные технологические навыки, которые позволят психологамкомпаративистам использовать автоматизацию и изготавливать оборудование специально для своих разновидностей и тем исследования.

Текст научной работы на тему «Automated Research in Comparative Psychology: Limitations and New Directions»

DOI 10.24411/1813-145X-2018-10173

УДК 159

Christopher Varnon A. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4826-0461 Harley Lang

https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4278-5083

Charles I. Abramson https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5114-8230

Automated Research in Comparative Psychology: Limitations and New Directions

Behavioral research is often enhanced by automated techniques, where experimental parameters and detection of behavior are controlled by electromechanical systems. Automated research promotes refinements in measurement, greater experimental control, longer durations of data collection, reduction of observer fatigue, and may permit new types of research to be conducted. In comparative psychology, use of automated techniques are often restricted to popular model organisms of fields such as behavior analysis and behavioral neuroscience. One factor contributing to this species-restriction may be the availability of automated research equipment, as most commercial research equipment is designed for rodents, and many researchers lack the skills required to create their own automated equipment. However, there are alternatives to commercial equipment, as some behavioral scientists have made available their own species-flexible, low-cost research equipment. In this paper, we provide three reviews. We first review recent trends in automated comparative psychology research, and then relate this to a second review on currently available automated research equipment. We also review affordable alternatives to commercial equipment that have been designed by behavioral scientists. Finally, we discuss useful technological skills that may allow comparative psychologists to take automation into their own hands and design equipment specific to their species and research topic.

Keywords: Behavior, behavioral research, automated techniques, behavioral scientists, automated equipment. Кристофер А. Варнон, Харли Ланг, Чарльз И. Абрамсон

Автоматизированные исследования в области сравнительной психологии: ограничения и новые направления

Исследование поведения часто проводится с помощью автоматизированных методов, где экспериментальные параметры и обнаружение поведения контролируются электромеханическими системами. Автоматизированное исследование способствует усовершенствованиям в измерении, большем экспериментальном контроле, обеспечивают более продолжительное хранение данных, уменьшение усталости наблюдателя, и могут применяться новые виды исследований. В сравнительной психологии использование автоматизированных методов часто ограничивается популярными моделями , такими как анализ поведения и поведенческая нейробиология. Одним из факторов, способствующих этому ограничению разновидностей, может быть доступность автоматизированного оборудования исследования, поскольку большая часть коммерческого оборудования исследования разработана для грызунов, и многие исследователи испытывают недостаток в навыках, требуемых для создания их собственного автоматизированного оборудования. Однако есть альтернативы коммерческому оборудованию, поскольку некоторые бихевиористы сделали доступным свое собственное гибкое к разновидностям, недорогостоящее оборудование для исследований. В данной статье мы представляем три отзыва. Мы сначала рассматриваем недавние тенденции в автоматизированном сравнительном исследовании психологии, и затем соотносим его со вторым анализом относительно доступности в настоящее время автоматизированного оборудования для исследований. Мы также рассматриваем доступные альтернативы коммерческому оборудованию, которые были разработаны бихевиористами. Наконец, мы обсуждаем полезные технологические навыки, которые позволят психологам-компаративистам использовать автоматизацию и изготавливать оборудование специально для своих разновидностей и тем исследования.

Ключевые слова: поведение, изучение поведения, автоматизированные методы, бихевиористы, автоматизированное оборудование.

One major goal of comparative psychology is to explore behavioral and cognitive abilities across species. Owing to a number of important historical figures, including Aristotle, Charles Darwin,

Nikolaas Tinbergen, and B. F. Skinner (Burghardt, 2009; Dewsbury, 1984; Jaynes, 1969; Lockard, 1971; Papini, 2003), comparative psychology has collected many methods and paradigms needed to

© Vamon Christopher A., Lang Harley, Abramson Charles I., 2018

make such comparisons. More recently, comparative research has been enhanced by use of automated procedures, in which recording behavioral variables and environmental manipulation is controlled by an electromechanical system. Ferster and Skinner's (1957) research in behavior analysis is one early example that demonstrated the usefulness of automated apparatuses, and subsequently inspired researchers across fields to make use of automation. For some reviews of the history of instrumentation in various aspects of psychological research see Abramson, (1994), Escobar (2014), and Sidowski (1996).

Automated techniques can provide several advantages compared to non-automated research: a wider swath of behavior can be measured simultaneously for any given organism, the duration of each measurement period can be extended indefinitely without concern for observer fatigue, and judgment errors can be better accounted for. As an example of the latter, in human research, environmental changes (e. g., Harris & Ciminero, 1978), behavioral patterns (e. g., Mash & Makohoniuk, 1975), and a host of other considerations adjust how we perceive behavior (see Kazdin, 1977 for a review), and it is likely these same issues occur across taxa. In addition to laboratory use, automation can also be used in the field to provide these powerful measurement benefits while still maintaining ecological validity (e. g., Craig et al., 2012; Morand-Ferron, Hamblin, Cole, Aplin, & Quinn, 2015).

Unfortunately, comparative psychology has often struggled to maintain a truly comparative focus (Burghardt, 2006), and this may be especially true for automated research as equipment is often available for only the most popular species. An additional barrier for automated comparative research is the high costs of commercial research equipment (Devarakonda, Nyguyen, & Kravitz, 2015; Hoffman, Song, & Tuttle, 2007; Pineno, 2014; Varnon & Abramson, 2013). However, a new researcher-driven movement in low-cost automation has permitted many laboratories to move away from reliance on commercial equipment. This may be the start of a new direction for truly comparative automated research. In this paper, we provide three reviews to investigate these topics. We first review current trends in species use and automation in comparative psychology. We then review currently available commercial research equipment and discuss how this may affect trends in comparative research. We also review low-cost alternatives to

commercial equipment, and discuss how they may provide new opportunities for research in comparative psychology.

Review 1: Species and Automation in Comparative Psychology

The comparative nature of the field suggests that a wide, balanced variety of species should be studied. However, early comparative psychologists instead found that rats were becoming a dominant research subject. By the 1920s, rats had become increasingly popular subjects with a variety of animal psychologists (Dewsbury, 1984; Logan, 1999). This infestation of rats continued into the 1980s, and was noted in journals such as Animal Behavior (Lown, 1975), and the Journal of Comparative Psychology (Gallup, 1989). From 1990 to 2004, the number of studies using rats in the Journal of Comparative Psychology dropped sharply, only to be replaced by non-human primates (Burghardt, 2006). Birds were consistently studied but the number of experiments involving reptiles, amphibians, fish, and invertebrates were very limited.

Many have voiced concern about the limited comparative perspective in comparative psychology. Critics have claimed that comparative psychology scientifically limits itself by focusing on a small proportion of species (Lockhard, 1971). This issue is so important that the most recent two editors of the Journal of Comparative Psychology, Josep Call and Gordon Burghardt, have both drawn attention to this problem. Josep Call suggested that a narrow set of subjects hinders our ability to reconstruct evolutionary paths for cognitive traits (Price, 2010), while Gordon Burghardt, stated that without information derived from a wide range of species we will have a "barren understanding of our own species" (Dingfelder, 2004, p. 51).

Method

To see if concerns of species use are still valid in recent years, we continued Gallup's (1989) and Burghardt's (2006) reviews of species use, and extended them to include both major comparative psychology journals, the International Journal of Comparative Psychology, and the Journal of Comparative Psychology. We further extended these reviews by also recording if the experiment used automated procedures. Our analysis focused on the years 2000 to 2016, and excluded editorials, theoretical papers, review papers, purely physiological research (e. g., body dimensions, facial features, heart rate, etc.), and human experiments. For each article, we record-

ed the details for each experiment separately, given that experiments in an article may differ substantially. We also recorded each species used in an article separately. All articles were manually accessed via institutional databases and publication websites, and then each experiment within each article was manually checked for inclusion by the second author. A random correspondence check was conducted to ensure accuracy of data. A total of 1,912 experiments, across 1,213 articles, were analyzed.

For each experiment, we recorded if hardware and/or software was used to automatically recorded behavior, or manipulate environmental variables. Experimental apparatuses were classified as manual if no forms of automation were used, or if the experimenters were required to actively moderate independent variables (e. g., presenting stimuli, controlling each putative reinforcer delivery with a button), and/or record dependent variables (e. g., pointing microphones, counting audio patterns, coding behaviors posthoc via audio or video record). Automated experiments were classified experiments as partially-automated (i. e., either the independent or dependent variables were automated) or fully-automated (i. e., both the independent and dependent variables were automated). For automated experiments, all automated measures and procedures were recorded. We excluded articles from the review if the authors did not clarify necessary details about the species, procedures, independent variable(s), or dependent variable(s).

Results and Discussion

Figure 1 displays the use of automation across the included articles. Between 2000 and 2016, we observed 1,470 manual apparatuses, 99 partially-automated apparatuses, and 326 fully-automated apparatuses. The number of partially-automated apparatuses may be slightly over-estimated given that this analysis required articles to specify automation in their methods; articles lacking sufficient details to classify as fully-automated were instead classified as partially-automated. The total use of automated apparatuses from 2000 to 2016 is small (23,12 % of surveyed articles), and the rate appears to be stable across this period.

Figure 2 displays a phylogram and stacked bar plot of species included in 10 or more experiments, both manual and automated, organized by phylogenetic order, then class. The phylogram was created in R Studio® (RStudio Te am, 2015) with the "ape" and "taxize" libraries (code available upon request).

2000 2002 2004 2006 2008 20Ю 2012 2014 2016 Year

Figure 1. A cumulative graph of automation in comparative psychology. The plot shows manual, partially-automated, and fully-automated apparatuses used in experiments found in the

International Journal of Comparative Psychology and the Journal of Comparative Psychology from 2000-2016

The listed species represent 66,4 % of the total species captured with this analysis. Rats (Rattus norvegicus, n = 197), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes, n = 170), and pigeons (Columba livia, n = 94) were the most frequently observed species. Of all orders, primates were most frequently presented (n = 440), while zebra fish (Danio rerio, n = 11) and honey bees (Apis mellifera, n = 13) were the only species representing non-mammalian and non-avian taxa. Automation (partial or full) was the most common with rats (n = 77), pigeons (n = 70), rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta, n = 39), chimpanzees (n = 31), and mice (Mus musculus, n = 19) while full automation was the most common with pigeons (n = 67), rats (n = 54), rhesus macaques (n = 34), chimpanzees (n = 21), and zebra finches (Taeniopy-gia guttata, n = 15). Overall, automation (partial or full) was used in 57 of the 280 listed species, which indicates that automated procedures are used less frequently than manual techniques.

We also noted some differences between the journals. The Journal of Comparative Psychology had more total experiments (1,521), more species represented (225), and more partially- or fully-automated experiments (312) than the International Journal of Comparative Psychology (391 experiments, 89 species, 113 automated experiments). However, the International Journal of Comparative Psychology had a higher proportion of species (22,76 %) and automated experiments (28,90 %) per total experiment than the Journal of Comparative Psychology (14,79 % species, 20,5 % automated).

Table 1 shows the behavioral responses that were recorded in three or more automated experiments, sorted by frequency and species. Pecking (n = 91)

was the most commonly observed response, and was seen primarily in pigeons. Lever pressing (n = 48) was the second most commonly observed response, and was seen primarily in rats. Joystick moving (n = 39) and touch screen use (n = 24) were the next most commonly observed responses, and were primarily observed in primates.

The results of our review on species use (both manual and automated) mirror the findings of Burghardt's (2006) review and show that the field is still relatively dominated by non-human primate research, though rat research is experiencing a resurgence. Overall, the lack of diversity in observed taxa demonstrates that the comparative literature continues to research a uniform conglomeration of species. As seen in previous reviews (Burghardt, 2006; Gallup, 1989), very few experiments were published involving reptiles, amphibians, fish, and invertebrates. Our review also found that automation was uncommon outside of a small number of taxa (rodents, pigeons, and primates), and given the potential benefits of automation, this might hinder our comparative understanding of behavior. The use of a limited selection of species in automated research might be caused, in part, by the available automated instruments offered by behavior research companies. This last concern is the topic of our next review.

Table 1

Behaviors Recorded in Three

Behavior Frequency Species

Touchscreen 15 Macaca mulatta

Touchscreen 5 Pongo abelii

Touchscreen 4 Papio papio

Review 2: Commercial Equipment

Method

In order to examine if the trends in automated comparative research are related to commercial offerings, we reviewed the online offerings of three major suppliers of commercial behavior research equipment; Lafayette Instrument Neuroscience (Lafayette, IN, lafayetteneuroscience.com), Med Associates Inc. (Fairfax, VT, med-associates.com), and Harvard Apparatus (Holliston, MA, harvardap-paratus.com; includes both Coulbourn Instruments and Panlab). As detailed archival records of offerings in the 2000s are not available, this review is not intended to be an inclusive outline of historical changes. Instead, it is intended to be a snapshot of current trends that may relate to recent trends in comparative psychology research. Every item on each vendor's website was manually checked for inclusion by the primary author in October, 2017.

In our analysis, we included any electromechanical devices that could present stimuli or record responses. However, we omitted physiological stimuli and responses (e. g., calorimeters, intracranial stimulation), video and audio measures that required manual analysis, and devices intended for human subjects. For some devices, a single vendor offered multiple variations of a device. To prevent the review from being inflated by near duplicates from the same vendor, we recorded all variations of a device as being the same device. For example, if a vendor offered separate tone generators for 2,800 Hz and 4,500 Hz tones, we simply recorded that vendor as offering a tone generator.

Findings were classified of our review into eight major categories: activity tests, mazes, stimulus devices, response devices, combined stimulus/response devices, reinforcement devices, aversive devices, and rodent-specific tests. Activity tests included activity wheels, automatic doors, feeder head entry sensors, food intake sensors, infrared activity monitors, infrared beams (individual pairs or grid systems), lickometers, place preference apparatuses (without shock), and water intake sensors. Mazes included circular runways, elevated plus mazes, radial arm mazes, T mazes, and Y mazes. Stimulus devices included single, dual, and triple light-emitting diode (LED) stimuli, olfactory systems, tone generators, and white nose generators. Response devices included handhold bars, keys, levers, Christopher A. Varnon, Harley Lang, Charles I. Abramson

or More Automated Experiments

Behavior Frequency Species

Calls 7 Rattus norvegicus

Calls 6 Hyla versicolor

Head bobbing 6 Zalophus californianus

Heart rate 4 Anser anser

Lever press 35 Rattus norvegicus

Lever press 8 Mus musculus

Lever press 5 Chinchilla laniger

Feeder entry 12 Rattus norvegicus

Licking 9 Rattus norvegicus

Move joystick 24 Macaca mulatta

Move joystick 9 Cebus paella

Move joystick 6 Papio papio

Nose poke 8 Rattus norvegicus

Nose poke 3 Mus musculus

Pecking 76 Columba livia

Pecking 8 Taeniopygia guttata

Pecking 4 Aratinga canicularis

Pecking 3 Aphelocoma californica

Perching 11 Poecile atricapillus

Perching 6 Taeniopygia guttata

Perching 3 Columba livia

Perching 3 Sturnus vulgaris

Proboscis extension 3 Apis mellifera

Room entry 9 Rattus norvegicus

push/pull knobs, nose poke holes, omnidirectional levers, and response wheels. Combined stimulus/response devices included liquid- crystal displays (LCDs), multicolor and pattern display keys, retractable levers with and without tricolor LEDs, and nose poke holes with one to three LEDs, olfactory inlets, and guillotine doors. Reinforcement devices included grain feeders, liquid dispensers, and pellet dispensers. Aversive devices included shock generators, shuttle boxes (with shock), and shockable running wheels. Rodent specific tests included catalepsy tests, forced swim tests, platform activity monitoring

tests, reaching chambers, rota-rod tests, rotameter tests, skilled reach grasp tests, sleep deprivation tests, startle chambers, tail flick analgesia tests, and tail suspension tests. Finally, we noted the intended species for each device. Although some equipment may be adaptable to multiple species, we recorded the species the devices was intended to be used with, as designated by the vendor. For this reason, we also omitted any devices that had no species suggestions.

Figure 2. A phylogram and stacked bar plot displaying species observed in 10 or more experiments in the International Journal of Comparative Psychology and the Journal of Comparative Psychology from 2000-2016. Black-shaded portions of the bar plot represent experiments with manual apparatuses, grey-shaded portions represent partially-automated apparatuses, and white-shaded portions represent fully-automated apparatuses. The percentage beside each bar plot shows the percent of

experiments that were automated (partial or full automation)

Results and Discussion

A summary of our findings can be seen in Table 2. We found that rodent equipment accounted for 79,39 % of all equipment. Even when not considering the 11,84 % of equipment designed for rodent-specific tasks, rodent equipment still accounted for 76,62 % of all offered equipment. Pigeon and primate equipment accounted for 8,77 % and 7,46 % of equipment, respectively. A meager 4,39 % of equipment was labeled for other species, including dogs, guinea pigs, and pigs. Although it is likely that some of the equipment offered could be used for species similar to the intended species, the focus of these research equipment companies is clearly on rodent models of behavior.

Table 2

Review of Commercial Research

Only five items matched our general search criteria but did not clearly specify a species, and so were excluded from the review in Table 2. The first item was a response wheel without species designation; other vendors offered a similar product for rats. The other four items were computer vision programs listed without species designations. Computer vision is a relatively new technology, where a computer program analyzes a still image or each frame of a video to determine the location or current behavior of an animal, in real time, or post hoc. Two programs we found detected freezing behavior in response to startling or aversive stimuli. One of the freeze-detecting programs was designated as for "rodent species." The other two programs tracked the movement and location of animals. One was designed to accompany water mazes designed for rats and mice, but the software itself had no species designation. It is likely these computer vision programs are adaptable for similarly sized species, but it is possible that the algorithms are also optimized for rodents or specific procedures.

We also noted some interesting trends in our review. Most problematic for comparative

psychology is that no equipment was available for reptiles, amphibians, fish, or invertebrates. It seems unlikely that equipment designed for mammals and birds could be easily used with other taxa. For example, the food and water reinforcement procedures for rats and pigeons may not be effective with reptiles, as temperature change may be much more effective motivator (Garrick, 1979; Kemp, 1969; Krochmal & Bakken, 2003; Krochmal, Bakken, & LaDuc, 2004; Place, Varnon, Craig, & Abramson, 2017). The aquatic environment of fish, and the small size of invertebrates also require special apparatus considerations that would likely make these taxa unsuitable for most commercial equipment. However, it should be noted that custom equipment for fish and invertebrates is possible, and has been created by individual research teams (e. g., fish: Valente, Huang, Portugues, & Engert 2012; invertebrates: Dinges, Varnon, Cota, Slykerman, & Abramson 2017). We also found that all vendors offered red stimulus lights for rodents. This is surprising given that rodents, and most non-primate mammals, do not have red color receptors (Jacobs, 2009). This is an important point considering how vital an understanding of subject species is for comparative research.

In our review, we also observed that prices were generally only available by request. However, most equipment tends to be very expensive. For example, simple operant conditioning equipment may cost between

$2,000 to $20,000 (Devarakonda et al., 2015; Hoffman et al., 2007; Pineno, 2014, Varnon & Abramson, 2013). Although we did not formally record data on human devices, we noted that one vendor offered a wooden pyramid puzzle for $150; we were able to find identical puzzles online for $10 to $20.

Although we cannot suggest a causal effect of equipment offerings on species use in comparative psychology, it is easy to see similarities in limited taxa diversity in both published automated research and available equipment. Comparing the species and automation trends in Figure 2 to the percent of equipment offered by species in Table 2 clearly shows that both are dominated by rodents. Similarly, both the species and response trends in Table 1 and the percent of equipment offered by species in Table 2 also show a strong bias towards rodents, birds, and primates. If trends in species use and automation in comparative psychology are indeed related to commercial research equipment offerings, then perhaps one solution to increase diversity in taxa studied is to explore low-cost automation alternatives created by individual research

Category Rat Mouse Pigeon Primate Other Total

Activity 20 19 1 1 1 42

Maze 9 8 0 0 0 17

Stimulus 11 11 7 5 4 38

Response 12 10 3 5 1 31

Stimulus/Response 13 13 7 2 2 37

Reinforcement 6 6 2 4 2 20

Aversive 8 8 0 0 0 16

Rodent 11 16 0 0 0 27

Total 90 91 20 17 10 228

Percent 39,47 39,91 8,77 7,46 4,39 100

laboratories. Our third and final review investigates such alternatives.

Review 3: Low-Cost Automation Alternatives

Many researchers have sought custom alternatives to commercial automation to obtain equipment specialized to their research and species, or simply to reduce costs. Behaviorists especially have a long history of creating their own automated equipment (for a review, see Escobar, 2014). One of the most well-known alternatives was the Walter/Palya experiment controller (Palya & Walter, 1993; Walter & Palya, 1984). Although the technology is now somewhat outdated, the Walter/Palya experiment controller was successfully used with a variety of species, including pigeons (Minervini & Branch, 2013), rats (Ranaldi, Ferguson, & Beninger, 1994), bees (Dinges et al., 2013), and rattlesnakes (Place et al., 2017), and is still used by several laboratories today. Such a flexible experiment controller could help increase the number of representative species studied in comparative psychology. In our final review, we examined several journals to see if similar modern alternatives have been developed.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Method

We reviewed the journals Animal Behaviour, Behavior Research Methods, International Journal of Comparative Psychology, Journal of Comparative Psychology, Journal of Neuroscience Methods, and Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, between the years of 2000 and 2016, for the terms apparatus, automat* (The asterisk is interpreted as a wild character by search engines and therefore returns any article with words that begin with "automat," such as "automation" and "automated."), experiment controller, microcontroller, and microprocessor. The time period for the review was selected to have the best chance to find articles describing modern technology. We selected articles that contained detailed, replicable, low-cost (i. e., < $1,500 USD sans computer), electromechanical devices that could automatically respond to and/or record behavior. Furthermore, to be included, the description of the device needed to be detailed enough as to permit reproduction in other, unaffiliated laboratories. For the purpose of capturing devices useful for automated comparative research, we excluded devices intended only for human use, devices that only detected virtual behavior of a subject at a personal computer (e. g., mouse clicks), devices that only recorded physiology, and devices that relied exclusively on outdated technology, primarily the parallel port. We also scanned the references of all articles for related works that fit the review's criteria. All articles were

manually accessed via institutional databases and publication websites, and then manually checked for inclusion by the primary and second authors. After the primary and secondary authors agreed on article inclusion, a random correspondence check was used ensure accuracy of data.

Results and Discussion

Only 11 articles met the criteria to be included in the analysis (see Table 3). It appears that many researchers are using microcontrollers, such as the Arduino family of development boards (Arduino; New York, NY) or the Parallax Propeller microcontroller (Parallax Inc.; Rocklin, CA), to automate experiments. In some cases, the microcontroller itself was solely responsible for automation, and could save data onto an onboard secure digital (SD) card that could be later transferred to a computer, making the system small and portable. In other cases, the microcontroller served as an interface between a laboratory computer and an experimental apparatus. Unfortunately, the species and response trends mirrored those observed in Figure 1 and Table 1. However, some of the devices show promise for truly comparative research.

The earliest device in this review, OpenControl (Aguiar, Mendon?a, & Galhardo, 2007) is free computer vision location-tracking and automation software for maze experiments. Although a species is not specifically stated, the article describes typical rodent experiments and provides rodent examples. OpenControl requires a computer with Visual Basic (Microsoft; Redmond, WA) to run the software, which offers a graphical user interface. During an experiment, the software tracks the location of a rodent in real-time, while the computer's parallel port interfaces with devices, such as levers, feeders, and doors. Such devices can be controlled based on the subject's location. Although the paper does not offer specific hardware, it does make suggestions for connecting an OpenControl system to commercial equipment through the parallel port. Unfortunately, parallel ports are somewhat outdated and are no longer standard on most computers. Only users with older computers will likely be able to make full use of OpenControl. However, the computer vision aspects of OpenControl may still be useful on newer computers that lack a parallel port.

Hoffman, Song, and Tuttle's (2007) Electronic Operant Testing Apparatus (ELTOPA), is an operant conditioning apparatus for birds featuring three keys, each with a bicolor LED, and a food delivery system. The ELTOPA is based on the PIC16F877A microcontroller (Microchip Technology; Chandler,

AZ) and can transmit data to a personal computer through a serial connection. Although the ELTOPA apparatus is impressive and potentially adaptable, the system appears to be designed specifically for birds and is not a general-purpose system.

Gess, Schneider, Vyas, and Woolley (2011) describe an auditory recognition training system (ARTSy) to train zebra finches in conspecific call-recognition tasks. ARTSy provides food reinforcement when the birds peck a target (detected by an infrared beam) on hearing the appropriate call. The authors suggest that system can be used with any species, but the paper primarily discusses a go-nogo procedure with zebra finches. ARTSy is controlled through a MATLAB (MathWorks; Natick, MA) program on a laboratory computer. Unfortunately, the MATLAB requirement may increase the price substantially for those that do not already have a license.

Varnon and Abramson (2013) describe the Propeller Experiment Controller (PEC), an experiment controller, driven by the Parallax Propeller microcontroller, that is heavily inspired by the Walter/Palya controller (Palya & Walter, 1993; Walter & Palya, 1984). The paper describes how to use the PEC for a teaching laboratory and covers use of over twenty programs in habituation, classical conditioning, and operant conditioning. Programs are suitable for both classroom demonstrations and research. Like the Walter/Palya controller, but unlike most other devices in our review, the PEC is not dedicated to a specific species or set of responses. Instead, the user is free to interface the PEC with a variety of other equipment; the paper also describes how to connect some common equipment, such as levers and stimulus lights. Due to this flexibility, the authors do not provide a specific price but suggest the core components cost less than $100 and describe several inexpensive options. Additionally, PEC is the only system in our review that offers a modular library of code dedicated to behavioral research in addition to programs written for specific experiments. Again, this is much in line with the Walter/Palya controller. The PEC saves data to an onboard SD card and does not require a computer for use, making it the first portable system in our review. Although the primary topic of this paper is devoted to teaching laboratories, other papers describe experimental use with diverse species such as horses (Craig, Varnon, Pollock, & Abramson, 2015), and honey bees (Dinges et al., 2017). Additional information on use of the PEC and other equipment is available on the primary author's website (CAVarnon.com).

Poddar, Kawai, and Olveczky (2013) describe a home-cage operant apparatus for rats that provides water reinforcement contingent on lever presses or moving a joystick. LEDs are used to signal the start of a trial, while a tone was used to signal availability of reinforcement. The system is designed to control many home-cage apparatuses simultaneously, and entails one or more client computers that control the apparatuses, a trainer computer that controls the client computers, and a master computer that controls the trainer computer over the internet using a graphical user interface. Although the hardware for the individual apparatuses is relatively inexpensive, at about $500, the cost increases substantially when adding the computers, and then averages to around $1400 an apparatus for a 48-apparatus setup.

The ArduiPod (Pineño, 2014) is an operant chamber for rats designed around the Arduino Uno. The ArduiPod requires an iPod Touch or iPhone (Apple Inc.; Cupertino, CA) as a touchscreen and stimulus device. It also features a simple water delivery system for reinforcement. Data can be saved by the iPod or iPhone, making the ArduiPod another portable device untethered to a computer.

Escobar and Pérez-Herrera (2015) released an Arduino-based experiment controller as an update of Escobar's earlier parallel port system (Escobar & Lattal, 2010). The primary offering of the Escobar and Pérez- Herrera (2015) controller is a Visual Basic program enabling a computer to control an Arduino-based apparatus. This system allows a user to run several schedules of reinforcement programs with one or two response devices. Escobar's website (analisisdelaconducta.net) also offers several rodent-related devices and programs. Although Escobar's work appears to be focused on rodents, this system is not a species-specialized apparatus, and could potentially be used with other species, if adequate equipment is designed. However, reliance on a personal computer with the Visual Basic program means that the system is not portable, nor is it currently expandable outside of supported experiment programs. The Escobar and Pérez-Herrera (2015) controller is available for about $200, depending on the equipment selected. The price, however, is only for the core experiment controller; no equipment for an animal is included.

Kuusela and Lamsa (2016) describe a simple artificial flower for use with bumblebees. The flower uses an infrared beam to detect when the bees enter a passageway, and a small drop of nectar can be delivered by a servo-actuated dipper. Each artificial flower costs around $10, with a $100 control unit (driven by an Arduino Mega 2560) able to control up to 32 flowers. The control unit then interfaces with a

computer. Although very inexpensive, the dipper feeder does not likely provide the same precision of feeding as other bee equipment that uses syringe pumps to deliver food (e. g., Sokolowski & Abramson, 2010).

Nyguyen, O'Neal, Bolonduro, White, and Kravitz (2016) describe a home cage feeding device for mice that delivers food pellets and tracks when the pellets are removed and consumed. The device is battery- powered and records data onto an onboard SD card making one of the most portable devices in our review. Unfortunately, the abilities of this apparatus are rather limited to recording feeding behavior, and so would likely be best suited as a component of a more involved experiment.

Ponce, Genecin, Perez-Melara, and Livingstone (2016) describe an automated apparatus to train rhesus macaques to participate voluntarily in neuroscience procedures requiring a primate chair. Although the apparatus was designed specifically to train macaques to enter the restraining chair, the automated reinforcement procedures could be used for other research. The apparatus uses a capacitance sensor to detect touch responses, and two ultrasonic sensors to detect proximity. It also can deliver water as a reinforcer and generate audio on a small speaker. The devices in the chair are controlled by an Arduino Uno, and data is transferred to a MATLAB program on a computer.

Most recently, Devarakonda, Nguyen, and Kravitz (2016), developed the Rodent Operant Bucket (ROBucket), an Arduino-based operant chamber for mice literally built inside a small, square bucket. The ROBucket records nose poke activity in three holes using infrared sensors, including a central hole where water can be automatically delivered. It also saves data to an onboard SD card which can be transferred to a computer at a later time, making the ROBucket another portable system.

Overall, many of these low-cost systems are promising, especially when funding is difficult to obtain. Although most are designed for common species and research methods, it is easy to see how they might be adapted to expand research in comparative psychology. Out of the systems we reviewed, it appears that the PEC (Varnon & Abramson, 2013) and the Escobar and Pérez-Herrera (2015) controller show the most promise for comparative work, and are both well-supported by the author's websites. The PEC appears to provide the most species- and method-flexible system, but does require the user to have more technical skills to make use of it, outside of its documented applications. Escobar's controller, and website,

appear to provide more traditional rodent equipment using an Arduino / Visual Basic system that may appeal to those with experience with these popular technologies. Any of the systems we reviewed could be useful in removing reliance on commercial equipment and returning control of research to those who conduct it.

In addition to these modern systems, readers may find many other papers and resources that would not fit the strict criteria of our search, but still provide useful descriptions and electronics diagrams of equipment. Some provide rich details sufficient for replication, others provide less details but still offer a good starting point for related projects. Even older papers may offer details that are still relevant to modern electronics and apparatus construction. Some designs include devices to automatically condition the proboscis extension reflex in honey bees (Abramson & Boyd, 2001), pellet dispensers for birds (Berkhoudt, van der Reijden, & Meijmans, 1987), multispecies feeding and watering devices (Crowder, Wilkes, & Huneycutt, 1964), infrared pellet delivery verifiers (Pinkston, Ratzlaff, Madden, & Fowler, 2008), feeding devices for sheep (Sandler, Van Gelder, Karas, & Buck, 1971), devices to record the foraging behavior of flying insects (Sokolowski & Abramson, 2010; Sokolowski, Disma, & Abramson, 2010), universal feeders (Takahashi, 1995), infrared beam response devices for fish (Uter, 1978), lickometers for rodents and insects (Wall, Walters, & England, 1972; Ford, Abramson, Sears, & Gutierrez, 2004), open-source syringe pumps (Wijnen, Hunt, Anzalone, & Pearce, 2014; cavar-non.com/syringepump), and open-source rodent behavior research systems (brody-wiki .princeton.edu/bcontrol).

Researchers and students interested in creating their own equipment might also consider hobby electronics and robotics vendors, such as Adafruit Industries (New York City, NY; adafruit.com), Parallax Inc. (Rocklin, CA; parallax.com), Pololu Robotics and Electronics (Las Vegas, NV; pololu.com), Servocity (Winfield, KS; servocity.com), and Sparkfun Electronics (Niwot, CO; sparkfun.com). The websites of these vendors are ripe with easy to use equipment and educational resources. On examining the offerings of hobby vendors, one will quickly see the vast possibilities brought by modern microcontrollers and microprocessors. Microcontrollers are a complete programmable system contained within a single chip, and often designed to interface with other input and output devices, including levers, LEDs, speakers, and feeders. Popular microcontrollers include the Parallax Propeller, and the ARM (ARM

Holdings; Cambridge, UK) and PIC microcontroller families; the Arduino is not technically a microcontroller itself, rather it is a development board and software system that makes use of various microcontrollers. A microprocessor is a small, programmable computer processor, which along with other components, can be used to build general purpose or specialized systems. Hobbyist microprocessor platforms include the Beaglebone (Beaglebone.org Foundation; Oakland Twp, MI) and the Raspberry Pi (Raspberry Pi Foundation; Cambridge, UK) microprocessor families. Generally, microcontrollers are better for low-level automation required by experiments, while microprocessors are better for higher-level tasks like user interfaces and displaying video.

Using a microcontroller or microprocessor, it would be easy to create many types of devices. For

Low-Cost Automation Devices

Another potentially useful technology is computer vision. As with more traditional, electromechanical forms of automation, expensive computer vision tools are purchasable, but some researchers are also creating new open-source solutions (e. g., Aguiar et al., 2007; Conklin, Lee, Schlabach, & Woods, 2015; Kane & Zamani, 2014). For readers interested in developing free computer vision options, we suggest researching the Python programing language (python.org), in conjunction with the scientific analysis package, SciPy (Jones et al., 2001), and computer vision library, OpenCV (Open Source Computer Vision Library, opencv.org). 252

example, in an operant apparatus, tri-color LEDs can be used as stimulus lights, piezo speakers can be used to play simple audio tones, and micro switches or infrared sensors can act as response devices. Likely the most difficult aspect of an operant chamber is the reinforcement device, however many of the previously mentioned papers provide detailed plans. Much of this custom apparatus work is possible at a very low cost, and only requires rudimentary skills in electronics and programming. Fortunately, the hobby electronics and robotics resources previous described offer more than adequate educational information suitable for a novice. With a moderate time-investment, it would be possible to implement or extend the low-cost automation alternatives described in our review (Table 3) or create new, specialized systems.

Table 3

General Discussion

Our review of trends in species use in comparative psychology shows that, even in recent years, comparative psychology does not take advantage of the vast diversity of potential subject species. This is unfortunate, considering that species-differences are often found when research investigates the behavior of less common subjects. For example, Craig et al. found that honey bees show different trends in responding under fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement than traditional mammalian and avian subjects (Craig, Varnon, Sokolowski, Wells, & Abramson, 2014). Bitterman (1965) also described differences in response Christopher A. Varnon, Harley Lang, Charles I. Abramson

Device Species Behavior Manipulation Hardware Price

Aguiar et al. (2007) Rat Location Any Parallel port $0

Hoffman et al. (2007) Bird 3 key pecks Food delivery, 3 bicolor LEDs PIC16F877A $300

Gess et al. (2011) Zebra finch Key peck Food, house lights, calls Custom circuit board $1,250

Varnon & Abramson (2013) Any Any Any Propeller <$100

Poddar et al. (2013) Rat Lever press / joystick LEDs, tones, water delivery PC via custom electronics $1,400

Pineño (2014) Rat Touchscreen Water delivery, video stimuli Arduino UNO + iOS $300

Escobar & Pérez-Herrera (2015) Rat 2 lever presses Food delivery, house lights Arduino UNO $200

Kuusela & Lamsa (2016) Bumble bee Flower entry Nectar delivery Arduino UNO $132

Nguyen et al. (2016) Mouse Food removal Food delivery Arduino Mega 2560 $350

Ponce et al. (2016) Rhesus macaque Touch, proximity Water delivery, audio stimuli Arduino Pro $230

Devarakonda et al. (2016) Mouse 3 nose pokes Water delivery Arduino Uno $150

patterns in spatial and visual tasks across primates, rats, pigeons, turtles, fish, cockroaches, and earthworms. Interestingly, rats with cortical deconstruction respond more like fish in some visual problems. Finding such species-differences may be important to progressing the field of comparative psychology, especially when they can also be related to physiological processes or evolutionary history.

When considering trends in automated research, use of a limited number of species is further exaggerated. It also appears that the trends in automation may be exacerbated by the limited offerings of commercial research equipment. However, we describe a new movement in affordable automation alternatives that has been created by researchers frustrated with limited or expensive commercial options. We also discuss resources to help interested individuals venture into the world of automation. It is our hope that this information may inspire current and future comparative psychologists to explore new areas in automation with non- traditional subject species.

Although automation techniques are powerful, it should be stressed that direct observation is also an important technique, especially when considering novel species, complex intra-species interactions (e. g., human-dog companionship) and novel research methods. Once direct observation has provided a good understanding of the topography and properties of a behavior, automation techniques may be used to efficiently capture relevant aspects of that behavior. In many cases, the easiest path to collecting data may be to design an experiment around available automation. However, the important characteristics of a behavior may not be those that are captured by the most popular technology. For example, while the count or rate of lever presses are traditional measures in rodent work, response force and duration have been shown to be more important in some rodent psychopharmacology research (Fowler, Filewich, & Leberer 1977; Fowler & Liou, 1994). By initially employing direct observation, it may be possible to design automation around the species and behavioral properties of interest.

In conclusion, we hope that this paper will help renew interest in studying nontraditional species, especially using automated procedures. Our collective efforts to better understand behavior can be enhanced with the addition of automated techniques where applicable, and will likely lead to new avenues for comparative research. We also hope that this paper may help inspire a new generation of

comparative psychologists, as the interest of undergraduate students are essential for the future of the field (Abramson, 2015).

Авторами представлен для публикации материал, ранее изданный: International Journal of Comparative Psychology. - Vol. 31. - 2018. - 01. - 01. - URL: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8gf4d5tx

Reference List

1. Abramson, C. I. (1994). A primer of invertebrate learning: The behavioral perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

2. Abramson, C. I., & Boyd, J. (2001). An automated apparatus for conditioning proboscis extension in honey bees, Apis mellifera L. Journal of Entomological Science, 36, 78-92.

3. Abramson, C. I. (2015). A crisis in comparative psychology: Where have all the undergraduates gone? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1500. doi:10.3389/fpsyg. 2015.01500.

4. Aguiar, P., Mendonga, L., & Galhardo, V (2007). OpenControl: A free opensource software for video tracking and automated control of behavioral mazes. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 166, 66-72. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth. 2007.06.020.

5. Berkhoudt, H., van der Reijden, D., & Meijmans, M. (1987). An automatic pellet dispenser for precise control of feeding topography in granivorous birds. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 435-438. doi: 10.1901/jeab. 1987.48-435

6. Bitterman, M. E. (1965). Phyletic differences in learning. American Psychologist, 20, 396-410.

7. Burghardt, G. M. (2006). Editorial: Journal of Comparative Psychology. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 77- 78. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.77

8. Burghardt, G. M. (2009). Darwin's legacy to comparative psychology and ethology. American Psychologist, 64, 102- 110. doi:10.1037/a0013385

9. Conklin, E. E., Lee, K. L., Schlabach, S. A., & Woods, I. G. (2015). VideoHacking: Automated tracking and quantification of locomotor behavior with open source software and off-the-shelf video equipment. Journal of Undergraduate Neuroscience Education, 13, A120-A125.

10. Craig, D. P. A., Grice, J. W., Varnon, C. A., Gibson, B., Sokolowski, M. B C., & Abramson, C. I. (2012). Social reinforcement delays in free-flying honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). PloS One, 7, e46729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone. 0046729.

11. Craig, D. P. A., Varnon, C. A., Pollock, K. L., & Abramson, C. I. (2015). An assessment of horse (Equus ferus caballus) responding on fixed interval schedules of reinforcement: An individual analysis. Behavioral Processes, 120, 1 - 13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc. 2015.08.006.

12. Craig, D. P. A., Varnon, C. A., Sokolowski, M. B. C., Wells, H., & Abramson, C. I. (2014). An assessment of fixed interval timing in free-flying honey bees (Apis mel-

lifera ligustica): An analysis of individual performance. PloS One, 9, e101262.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone. 0101262.

13. Crowder, W. F., Wilkes, W. P., & Huneycutt, B. D. (1964). Simple motor-driven devices for feeding and watering animals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 313-314. doi:10.1901/jeab. 1964.7313

14. Devarakonda, K., Nguyen, K. P., & Kravitz, A. V. (2015). ROBucket: A low cost operant chamber based on the Arduino microcontroller. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 503-509.

15. Dewsbury, D. (1984). Comparative psychology in the twentieth century. Stroudsburg, PA: Hutchinson Ross Inc.

16. Dinges, C. W., Avalos, A., Abramson, C. I., Craig, D. P. A., Austin, Z. M., Varnon, C. A,.... Wells, H. (2013). Aversive conditioning in honey bees (Apis mellif-era anatolica): A comparison of drones and workers. Journal of Experimental Biology, 216, 4124-4134. doi:10.1242/jeb. 090100.

17. Dinges, C. W., Varnon, C. A., Cota, L. D., Slykerman, S., & Abramson, C. I. (2017). Studies of learned helplessness in honey bees (Apis mellifera ligus-tica). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 43, 147-158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000133

18. Dingfelder, S. F. (2004). Burghardt to head the Journal of Comparative Psychology. Monitor on Psychology, 35, 51.

19. Escobar, R. (2014). From relays to microcontrollers: The adoption of technology in operant research. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 40, 127-153.

20. Escobar, R., & Pérez-Herrera, C. A. (2015). Low-cost USB interface for operant research using Arduino and Visual Basic.

21. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103, 427-435. doi:10.1002/jeab. 135 Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York, NY: Prentice Hall.

22. Ford, J., Abramson, C. I., Sears, N., & Gutierrez, F. (2004). A low-cost drinkometer circuit suitable for insects and other organisms. Psychological Reports, 94, 1137-1143. doi:10.2466/pr0.94.3c. 1137-1143

23. Fowler, S. C., & Liou, J. R. (1994). Microcata-lepsy and disruption of forelimb usage during operant behavior: Differences between dopamine D1 (SCH-23390) and D2 (raclopride) antagonists. Psychopharma-cology, 115, 24-30.

24. Fowler, S. C., Filewich, R. J., & Leberer, M. R. (1977). Drug effects upon force and duration of response during fixed- ratio performance in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 6, 421-426.

25. Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1989). Editorial. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 3.

26. Garrick, L. D. (1979). Lizard thermoregulation: Operant responses for heat at different thermal intensities. Copeia, 2, 258-266.

27. Gess, A., Schneider, D. M., Vyas, A. & Woolley, S. M. N. (2011). Automated auditory recognition training and testing.

28. Animal Behaviour, 82, 285-293. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav. 2011.05.003

29. Harris, F. C., & Ciminero, A. R. (1978). The effect of witnessing consequences on the behavioral recordings of experimental observers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 513-521.

30. Hoffman, A. M., Song, J., & Tuttle, E. M. (2007). ELOPTA: A novel microcontroller-based operant device. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 776-782. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192968.

31. Jacobs, G. H. (2009). Evolution of colour vision in mammals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2957-2967. doi: 10.1098/rstb. 2009.0039

32. Jaynes, J., (1969). The historical origins of 'ethology' and 'comparative psychology'. Animal Behaviour, 17, 601-606. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(69)80001-1

33. Jones, E., Oliphant, E., & Peterson, P., et al. (2001). SciPy: Open Source Scientific Tools for Python. Retrieved from http://www.scipy.org/

34. Kane, S. A., & Zamani, M. (2014). Falcons pursue prey using visual motion cues: new perspectives from animal-borne cameras. Journal of Experimental Biology, 217, 225-234. doi: 10.1242/jeb. 092403

35. Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and complexity of assessment: The ABCs of reliability. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 141-150.

36. Kemp, F. D. (1969). Thermal reinforcement and thermoregulatory behaviour in the lizard Dipsosaurus dorsalis: an operant technique. Animal Behaviour, 17, 466-451.

37. Krochmal, A. R., & Bakken, G. S. (2003). Thermoregulation is the pits: Use of thermal radiation for retreat site selection by rattlesnakes. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206, 2539-2545.

38. Krochmal, A. R., Bakken, G. S., & LaDuc, T. J. (2004). Heat in evolution's kitchen: Evolutionary perspectives on the functions and origin of the facial pit of pitvi-pers (Viperidae: Crotalinae). Journal of Experimental Biology, 207, 4231-4238.

39. Kuusela, E., & Lamsa, J. (2016). A low-cost, computer-controlled robotic flower system for behavioral experiments.

40. Ecology and Evolution, 6, 2594-2600. doi:10.1002/ece3.2062

41. Lockard, R. B. (1971). Reflections on the rise and fall of comparative psychology: Is there a lesson for us all? American Psychologist, 26, 168-179.

42. Logan, C. A. (1999). The altered rationale for the choice of a standard animal in experimental psychology: Henry H. Donaldson, Adolf Meyer, and «the» albino rat. History of Psychology, 2, 3-24.

43. Lown, B. A. (1975). Comparative psychology 25 years after. American Psychologist, 30, 858-859.

44. Mash, E. J., & Makohoniuk, G. (1975). The effects of prior information and behavioral predictability on observer accuracy. Child Development, 46, 513-519.

45. Minervini, V & Branch, M. (2013). Tolerance to cocaine's effects following chronic administration of a dose without detected effects on response rate or pause. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 100, 316-332.

46. Morand-Ferron, J. Hamblin, S., Cole, E. F., Aplin, L. M., & Quinn, J. L. (2015). Taking the operant paradigm into the field: Associative learning in wild great tits. PLoS ONE, 10, e0133821. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone. 0133821

47. Nguyen, K. P., O'Neal, T. J., Bolonduro, O. A., White, E., & Kravitz, A. V. (2016). Feeding experimentation device (FED): A flexible open-source device for measuring feeding behavior. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 267, 108-114.

48. Palya, W. L., & Walter, D. E. (1993). A powerful, inexpensive experiment controller or IMB PC interface and experiment control language. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 25, 127-136.

49. Papini, M. R. (2003). Comparative psychology. In S. F. Davis (Ed.), Handbook of Research Methods in Experimental Psychology (pp. 211-240). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

50. Pineno, O. (2014). ArduiPod Box: A low-cost and open-source Skinner box using an iPod touch and an Ar-duino microcontroller. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 196-205.

51. Pinkston, W. J., Ratzlaff, K. L., Madden, G. J., & Fowler, S. C. (2008). An inexpensive infrared detector to verify the delivery of food pellets. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 249-255. doi: 10.1901/jeab. 2008.90-249

52. Place, A. J., Varnon, C. A., Craig, D. P. A., & Abramson, C. I. (2017). Exploratory investigations in operant thermoregulation in western diamond-backed rattlesnakes (Crotalus atrox). In M. J. Dreslik, W. K. Hayes, S. J. Beaupre, & S. P. Mackessy (Eds.), The Biology of Rattlesnakes (pp. 213-227). Rodeo, NM: ECO Herpetological Publishing and Distribution.

53. Poddar, R., Kawai, R., & Olveczky, B. P. (2013). A fully automated high-throughput training system for rodents. PLoS ONE, 8: e83171. doi:10.1371/journal.pone. 0083171

54. Ponce, C. R., Genecin, M. P., Perez-Melara, G., & Livingstone, M. S. (2016). Automated chair-training of rhesus macaques, Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 263, 75-80.

55. Price, M. (2010). An evolution of comparative psychology. Monitor on Psychology, 41, 73.

56. Ranaldi, R., Ferguson S., & Beninger, R. J. (1994). Automating the generation and collection of rate-frequency functions in a curve-shift brain stimulation rewarding paradigm. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 53, 163-172.

57. RStudio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA. Available from http://www.rstudio.com/

58. Sandler, B. E., Van Gelder, G. A., Karas, G. G., & Buck, W. B. (1971). An operant feeding device for sheep. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 9596. doi:10.1901/jeab. 1971.15-95

59. Sidowski, J. B. (Ed.). (1966). Experimental methods and instrumentation in psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

60. Sokolowski, M. B. & Abramson, C. I. (2010). From foraging to operant conditioning: A new computer-controlled Skinner box to study free-flying nectar gathering behavior in bees. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 188, 235-242.

61. Sokolowski, M. B. C., Disma, G., & Abramson, C. I. (2010). A paradigm for operant conditioning in blow flies (Phormia terrae novae Robineau-Desvoidy). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 81-89. doi:10.1901/jeab. 2010.93-81.

62. Takahashi, M. (1995). A low-cost universal feeder. Behavior Research Methods, 27, 127-136. doi:10.3758/BF03203624 Uter, T. G. (1978). A fish activity-monitoring device using a low-power infrared photo-beam. Behavior Research Methods,

63. 10, 411-414. doi:10.3758/BF03205164

64. Valente, A., Huang, K., Portugues, R., & Engert, F. (2012). Ontogeny of classical and operant learning behaviors in zebrafish. Learning and Memory, 19, 170-177. doi:10.1101/lm. 025668.112

65. Varnon, C. A., & Abramson, C. I. (2013). The propeller experiment controller: Low-cost automation for classroom experiments in learning and behavior. Comprehensive Psychology, 2, 1-18. doi:10.2466/07.08.IT.2.2.

66. Wall, A. M., Walters, G. C., & England, R. S. (1972). The lickometer: A simple device for the analysis of licking as an operant. Behavior Research Methods, 4, 320-322. doi:10.3758/BF03207315

67. Walter, D. E., & Palya, W. L. (1984). An inexpensive experiment controller for stand-alone applications or distributed processing networks. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16, 125-134.

68. Wijnen B., Hunt, E. J., Anzalone, G. C., & Pearce, J. M. (2014). Open-Source Syringe Pump Library. PLoS ONE, 9, e107216. doi:10.1371/journal.pone. 0107216

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.