Научная статья на тему 'The ethology of protozoa and the adaptive space hypothesis: a heuristic approach to the biology of these eukaryotic, unicellular organisms'

The ethology of protozoa and the adaptive space hypothesis: a heuristic approach to the biology of these eukaryotic, unicellular organisms Текст научной статьи по специальности «Биологические науки»

CC BY
144
38
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
Журнал
Protistology
Область наук
Ключевые слова
BEHAVIOUR / CILIATES / ADAPTIVE BIOLOGY

Аннотация научной статьи по биологическим наукам, автор научной работы — Banchetti Rosalba, Erra Fabrizio

The analysis of the behaviour of the ciliated protozoa led us to explore different fields of their adaptive biology. The neuro ethological and the eco ethological studies in our Lab are shortly considered in their aspects of uniqueness (due to the biological peculiarities of protozoa) to support the proposal of a new hypothesis: the adaptive space of the protozoa we studied, a unique space defined by their cellular, organismic and environmental parameters. The heuristic value of this hypothesis is then experimentally tested with the study of the case of the double organisms of Oxytricha bifaria, which represent an allomorphic living state of the species. The possible adaptive significance of this allomorphic state is discussed.

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.
iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.
i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.

Текст научной работы на тему «The ethology of protozoa and the adaptive space hypothesis: a heuristic approach to the biology of these eukaryotic, unicellular organisms»

Protistology 3 (1), 58-68 (2003)

Protistology

The ethology of Protozoa and the “adaptive space” hypothesis: a heuristic approach to the biology of these eukaryotic, unicellular organisms

Rosalba Banchetti and Fabrizio Erra*

Department of Etology, Ecology and Evolution, University of Pisa, Italy

Summary

The analysis of the behaviour of the ciliated protozoa led us to explore different fields of their adaptive biology. The neuro-ethological and the eco-ethological studies in our Lab are shortly considered in their aspects of uniqueness (due to the biological peculiarities of protozoa) to support the proposal of a new hypothesis: the “adaptive space” of the protozoa we studied, a unique space defined by their cellular, organismic and environmental parameters. The heuristic value of this hypothesis is then experimentally tested with the study of the case of the double organisms of Oxytricha bifaria, which represent an allomorphic living state of the species. The possible adaptive significance of this allomorphic state is discussed.

Key words: behaviour, ciliates, adaptive biology

The first and fundamental characteristic of every protozoon is its double nature. It is at the same time a perfect eukaryotic cell (functional unit) and a complete organism (adaptive unit). Protozoa, during their long existence, have had to face an extremely wide variety of biological and environmental challenges, and have survived thanks to different adaptive tricks. Such tricks are even found in the world of metazoa, even if

* We would like to dedicate this paper to the memory of Prof. Nicola Ricci, who introduced us in the field of ethology of ciliates. We’ll remember this exuberant teacher and researcher for his friendship and his sincere enthusiasm. The affection and respect, in which he was held, will endure.

‘‘recycled’’ for new adaptive purposes. As an example of this process of re-use one could mention the ability of protozoa to change form and functional state (in a genetically codified and reversible manner), in order to deal, over long periods of time, with different environmental opportunities or challenges.

Typical is the case of Oxytricha bifaria, a freshwater ciliate, capable of encysting (reorganizing and reducing it own volume to about 1/20 of normal) to avoid extreme conditions of temperature or dryness (Ricci et al., 1985) or producing carnivorous giants (Ricci and Banchetti, 1993) whenever there is a superabundance ofpotential prey. O. bifaria, by differentiating the giants, radically changes form and function adaptively. The giants are survival forms of the species when dense

© 2003 by Russia, Protistology

populations undergo severe starvation. They are carnivorous predators, namely differentiated states of the species feeding on new pabula (ciliates instead of bacteria). In this way they reduce the intraspecific competition for the normal bacterial food and from primary consumers become secondary consumers. The successive steps of the giant formation in O. bifaria are successive phases of a primigenial cell differentiation, that is a reversible phenomenon. This potential tool was the first realization of what metazoa recycled later as irreversible cell differentiation. Irreversibility was added only when multicellularity was reached (Ricci and Banchetti, 1993).

The second characteristic is that protozoa are ‘‘small’’, the average dimensions varying from 30 to 300 ^m. This simple ‘‘being small’’ has consequences for their biology. (A) As discussed by Purcell (1977), organisms of these dimensions live at low Reynolds numbers, namely they experience a peculiar aqueous environment, where, no inertia conditioning their locomotion, the visco-elastic forces mainly affect it (we will discuss in another paragraph the consequences of this property of protozoa). (B) All of the environments are fragmented for them into a myriad of environmental micropatches, identified by the infinite combinations of the minimum variations in space and time of the various environmental parameters, which support the local biodiversity of protozoa. (C) Whenever there are optimal physiological states and favorable external conditions, they multiply at extremely high reproductive rates in soils, rivers and seas (Finlay et al., 1988; Finlay, 1990). (D) Protozoa, the first eukaryotes to reach the niches of primary and secondary consumer (Gould et al., 1977) achieved the first ‘‘complete ecology’’. These organisms are the successive steps of the microbial food chain, that is the “microbial loop”. They are large enough to be proper food for both larvae and adults of larger metazoa that belong to the “macrobial loop” (Azam et al., 1983; Fenchel, 1987).

From Jennings (1906), the first modern scientist to study the ‘‘behaviour of lower organisms’’ with a naturalistic, experimental mentality, the behaviour of ciliated protozoa has been increasingly seen as an object of study of great importance for anyone interested in discovering new pieces of the great jigsaw puzzle of nature (Ricci, 1981; Ricci et al., 1995; Leonildi et al., 1998).

A short preface shoul be given to review the basic elements of an ethogram: all the species studied by us creep on the substrate along a piecewise linear path formed by three Long Lasting Elements, LLE (they last one to several seconds: rightward arcs, A+, segments, S, and leftward arcs, A-) and four Short Lasting Elements, SLE (they last about 1/10s:

Continuous Trajectory Change, CTC, Smooth Trajectory Change, STC, Rough Trajectory Change, RTC, and Side Stepping Reaction, SSR), which mediate the passage from one LLE to the next.

Here we would like to put forward the study of the ethology of protozoa as a precious instrument: we will discuss what such a study can contribute to our knowledge of protozoa..

1. The neuro-ethological approach

In protozoa the nature of perfect eukaryotic cell and complete organism coincide: this makes them a convenient material for neuro-ethological investigations, namely for the study of the relationships between behavioural patterns and electrical activities underlying them (Hoyle, 1970; Usherwood and Newth, 1975; Ewert, 1976; Kandel, 1976). The very character of neuro-ethology leads it to concern with the analysis of the relations between the manifold organizational levels that have to interact for the expression of behaviour: from the cellular organelle (for example: the cell membrane), to the cell (for example: the neuron), to the tissue (for example: the neural network), to the organ (for example: the ganglion), to the system (for example: the nervous system), and finally to the phenomenon that integrates and expresses all this, namely the behaviour. Huber and Markl (1983) and Cliff (1991) stated that a neuroethological approach iwill be all the more precious the greater is the distance between the interacting levels analyzed. The use of ciliates in neuroethology was proposed (Ricci, 1992) on the basis of three facts: a) the electrophysiology of ciliates is a very well-known field of protozoology (Machemer and Teunis, 1996); b) the behaviour is well studied and understood (Ricci, 1989, 1990); c) the double nature of cell and organism that characterizes protozoa makes possible the direct study of the electrophysiology of a single cell (the lowest neuro-ethological level) and its corresponding behavioural states (the highest neuro-ethological level).

Direct attribution of behaviour of freely moving organisms to spontaneously occurring electrical events has already been described in Bursaridium difficile (Berg and Sand, 1994). In our laboratory an attempt was made to find in Euplotes vannus spontaneous behavioural patterns and then identify their electrical equivalents by means of a computerized video-track analyzing system to correlate beating of cirri to the membrane potential of a freely walking organism (Lueken et al., 1996). It was demonstrated that the creeping velocity of organisms varies spontaneously and periodically (Slow-and-Fast pattern) in quite a regular manner (T=0.6-0.8s) (Fig. 1A,B); the relative memb-

35 J __________________________________

0 2 4 6 £

tone (s)

Fig. 1. (A) A track-type of Euplotes vannus is shown as the succession of the positions of the baricentre of the organism in the succession of single photograms of the video-recording; (B) the variation in time of the velocity of the organism from point to point; (C) an analogous pattern of the membrane potential of this ciliate, spontaneously recurrent in the absence of stimuli.

rane potential in the same conditions oscillated in a similar way (T=0.7s) (Fig.1C). This observation seems to suggest that spontaneous bursts of depolarizations are responsible for the slow-and-fast rhythm in freely walking cells, by affecting beating of the cirri so that cells decelerate during the depolarizing level, accelerate during the repolarization, and reach full speed during the common potential level (-25-30 mV): this represents the system of control of the velocity.

The correspondence in frequencies of the slow-and-fast rhythm in walking cells (1.3/s) and of the depolarization pattern of the membrane potential (1.4/s) was confirmed in the wide majority (80%) of track recordings. Whenever the membrane potential assumed values > -15 mV, the beat of both fronto-ventral and transverse cirri is inverted: this value seems to be a threshold controlling the execution of a Side Stepping Reaction (SSR). In SSR deceleration ends in a complete stoppage that is succeeded by backward jerking. In recordings of the free-running membrane potential, SSRs correspond to long-lasting depolarizations. Resumption of forward running is the consequence of repolarization.

To different states of the membrane potential of the ciliate correspond different states of functioning of the motor organelles that determine different behaviour patterns. These results demonstrate that “complex behaviours have their roots in the principles of sensory-motor coupling” (Machemer and Teunis, 1996) and that in protozoa each phenomenon can be analyzed in

its manifold cellular or orga-nismic meanings by means of neuro-ethology.

2. The eco-ethological

APPROACH

The eco-ethological approach to protozoa may, so to say, “putt us in their shoes”, and lead us to a better understadning of their peculiar environment and equally peculiar environmental biology.

Fenchel’s book (1987) had marked the beginning of a real ecology of protozoa, to throw light on the numerous peculiarities and the importance for the flow of energy and substance that these organisms mediate in any environment. The simultaneous development of etholo-gical studies of protozoa, demonstrating the close link between some of their adaptive characteristics and environmental peculiarities, indicated that the eco-ethological approach was also possible for these organisms (Fig. 2) (Ricci, 1992a, 1996).

This first eco-ethological research enabled us to discover a vast microhabitat ideal for the settlement of hypotrich ciliates: the bacterized Water-Air-Interface, WAI (Ricci et al., 1991). The behavioural analysis led us to study the movements of O. bifaria in contact with the WAI: if it is free from bacteria, O. bifaria tends to avoid the WAI, and, if moving on it, never performs any trajectory change but the Continuous Trajectory Change. Furthermore, O. bifaria moves by sliding and drifting obliquely along rightward arcs (A+), with short radii (=250 ^m vs. 890 ^m of controls), wide central angles (P=140o vs. 50o of controls), and centers in slightly different positions with respect to each other (Fig. 3, upper left part). This slow (v=220 jwm/s vs. 550 ^m/s of controls), constant and monotonous locomotion seems to suggest that colonization of the WAI is brought about by this species mainly according a bidimensional dispersion mediated by the creeping itself, rather than by the swimming.

On the contrary, whenever the interface is colonized by a bacterial film, the population settles in it leaving the free interface immediately. O. bifaria moves (Fig. 4, upper right part) on the substrate at =550 ^m/s, along normal tracks formed by a succession of wide radius (= 890 jwm), leftward arcs (A-), regularly

Fig. 2. The interactive cycle between environment and organism: the stimuli that characterize the peculiar environment of protozoa, decoded by the sensory organelles, elaborated by the system-organism, elicit a response expressed by behaviour that is capable of dealing with the internal/external needs, changing the position of the organism in the environment itself and «adjusting» the stimuli within appropriate values.

traveled tangentially to the arc itself and connected by the series of trajectory change reactions, CTC, STC, RTC, and SSR), which enable the population to react adaptively to the widest possible range of external challenges as well as to internal needs.

The world of O. bifaria is actually formed by all the surfaces covered by a bacterial film (Fig. 3, lower

right part): from the different components of the bottom, to the vegetable stems rising from it and emerging from the water, to the bacterized areas of WAI which the hypotrichs reach by walking directly on the stems without even having to swim. This suggests that all stagnant waters (Armentano, 1980) are an ideal environment for a trophic chain that is as simple as it

Fig. 3. The features of the behaviour of Oxytricha bifaria at the interface between water and air, free from bacteria (left part of the diagram) or covered by bacterial film (right part of the diagram). In the upper panels the behavioural parameters are shown with their measurements, the typical arcs and the typical tracks followed by O. bifaria in the two conditions.

is specialized: 1) the producers (bacteria covering all rivorous hypotrich ciliates which move and feed on

the substrates), 2) the primary consumers (bacte- them) and 3) the secondary consumers (other, few,

Fig. 4. The accumulation of ciliates under the surface between water and air ‘‘blocked’’ in its free and constant oscillation (enlarged diagram in the lower panel) by (1) bacterial films, (2) floating objects or (3) menisci induced by objects interacting with the air-water surface .

carnivore ciliates and, for the most part, metazoa, such as the larvae of some Diptera, commonly known as meniscus midges, so called because their larvae live in the meniscus of the water and feed only on ciliates living at the WAI level (Ricci, 1992a).

Even more unexpected were the results of the study of the behaviour of ciliated protozoa in shallow waters bodies (0-2 cm), such as temporary water wells, lateral loops of streams and rivulets, vast areas of shore of ponds, marshes and lakes (Fig. 4, upper part): this type of environment is as favorable to the development of abundant populations of ciliates (because of the absence, or near-absence, of hydrodynamism, because of the temperature, dissolved oxygen at optimal levels, because of the bacteria never representing a limiting factor), as it is dangerous for them because of the risk of sudden, lethal periods of drought. In this environment it was observed that ciliates tend to collect in high numbers of individuals that are almost immobile on the bottom in correspondence with particular areas of WAI (Ricci et al., 1992). Their distinguishing characteristic is that ofbeing in some way “immobilized” (Fig. 4, lower part), whether this happens because of the development of thick, bacterial films (Fig. 4, lower part, 1), or because of the presence of floating objects (mainly leaves; Fig. 4, lower part, 2), or because of the establishment of menisci in a state of tension between the various objects that interfere with the WAI itself (Fig. 4, lower part, 3, between the leaves).

The analysis of behaviour in the areas of accumulation revealed that the 10-fold higher cell density in the areas ‘‘protected’’ from above is due to several factors working synergistically: (a) the oxytrichas get there by chance, (b) they move there at a reduced velocity, (c) they follow trajectories interrupted by repeated Side Stepping Reactions, such as to keep the organism in the area itself, and (d) they perform clear SSR whenever they creep outwards to the limit of the favourable area (Ricci and Erra,

1995). To explain this effect the nature of the WAI was studied: it was seen that it is in a state of continuous “rain” of microvibrations produced by the WAI itself and that these microvibrations are propagated downwards, disturbing tonically the hypo-trichs exposed to them.

Ciliates accumulate under a floating leaf or under a thick, floating, bacterial film due to a behaviour which enables them to avoid the mechanical stimuli transmitted by the WAI. This behaviour gives the ciliates four adaptive advantages: (a) they find extensive

areas rich in bacteria; (b) they find themselves in an environment that, in the event of drought, will remain humid for a longer time (because of the protection offered by the leaf itself), thus leaving the organism time to undergo differentiation into resistance cysts and survive; (c) they form dense populations that permit the meeting and conjugation of organisms of different mating-types; (d) they undergo differentiation, which is known to be strictly cell density-dependent (Ricci and Banchetti, 1993), of gigantic forms, capable of enabling the species, normally a primary (bacteri-vorous) consumer to exploit a new pabulum (the other ciliates) typical of a secondary consumer (carnivore). This second eco-ethological study has made possible the integrated reading of different, apparently unconnected, phenomena, in a single, adaptive perspective.

The eco-ethological approach has recently proved to be useful also in a new, applied field of research, that of so-called etho-bio-monitoring, proposed by various authors since 1980 (Kittredge, 1980; Olla et al., 1980). Gray (1995) studied the behaviour of the salmon as the adaptive interface between organism and environment, capable of revealing if and how far the environment has been modified. The use of the analysis of the behaviour of protozoa, proposed in this sense by Ricci (1995), led us to demonstrate that the behaviour of Tetrahymena pyriformis and T. malaccensis was able to

Fig. 5. The behaviour of protozoa, by connecting the organism to the environment, functions as a link between the three axes (x: cell; y: organism; z: environment), thus identifying an ‘‘adaptive space’’.

reveal the presence of infinitesimal quantities of insulin, a factor released by such species into the water medium and interpreted as a stimulus-guide for a positive chemiotropism to allow the meeting of co-specific organisms dispersed in the environment, similarly to what cAMP does for Dyctiostelium discoideum. This hormone, at a final concentration of 10-6 M for 60 min, affected differently the behaviour of T. pyriformis and T. malaccensis. In T. pyriformis insulin induced significant increase in the velocity of each Long Lasting Elements, while the Short Lasting Elements were unaffected (Mugnaini et al., 1995). This observation suggested that T. pyryformis changed its behaviour markedly under the influence of the “memory” of the treatments rather than under the treatments themselves.

The analysis of the creeping of E. vannus demonstrated that minimum concentrations of Hg++ (2 ^M) are able to affect strikingly the behaviour of the studied populations. The creeping of a treated organism is very fluid, uniform, continuous and constant. A behavioural pattern typical of E. vannus is the ‘‘Slow-Down’’: the organisms periodically (every 1-2 s) slow down for a very short time (0.4-0.5 s), reducing their average speed from 450 ^m/s to values ranging around 180-230 ^m/ s. A second typical pattern of E. vannus is the common performance of 3-4 normal SSRs in quick succession. Both typical patterns disappear after Hg++ treatment. The more subtle effects of Hg++ are only detectable through the analysis of the ethogram: the average rotation angle of the organisms performing the SSR duplicates, with respect to the control (163° vs. 84°); the normal SSR tends to disappear (8.5% vs. 46%), while the prolonged SSR is performed by E. vannus only after treatment with Hg++; finally the average velocity of the organisms is reduced from 450 to 340 ^m/s when Hg++ is applied for more than 10 min (Ricci et al. 1997). Thus, it can be stated that the study of the behaviour of protozoa can reveal the presence even of very small concentrations of pollutants.

3. The “adaptive space”: a new heuristic hypothesis

The neuro-ethological approach demonstrated that only for protozoa is there really a plan, identified by the two axes x (cell biology) and y (organismic biology), on which it is possible to carry out the analysis of the phenomena of their biology. The eco-ethological approach proved that the behaviour of protozoa is a sort of link-up between organismic biology (axis y) and environmental biology (axis z) (Fig. 5). On the basis of our results on Euplotes vannus, Oxytricha bifaria, Tetrahymena pyriformis and T. malaccensis, we would suggest the existence of an ‘‘adaptive space’’, identified by cell biology, organismic biology and environmental

biology, connected in a privileged fashion by a single phenomenological link their behaviour. This hypothesis of adaptive space would have a heuristic value, because it would make possible to ‘‘break down’’ any phenomenon of the biology of protozoa into its respective meanings on the three axes (cell, organismic, and environmental) and to proceed to an adaptive interpretation.

As an experimental example to show the correctness of this hypothesis let us consider the ‘‘doubleorganism’’ (doublet) of O. bifaria, so-called because formed by two organisms united by their dorsal regions: it is normally interpreted as the result of an abortive transversal binary division, as is well-known in O. hymenostoma and in many other species of ciliates: Euplotes, Blepharisma, etc. An initial study of the doublet of O. bifaria (Banchetti and Ricci, 1986) had led to a description of its cycle of development, consisting of five successive morphological stages, the first and the third of which are also able to undergo a regular cell cycle and reproduce by dividing themselves, demonstrating an absolutely functional underlying biology. Stage I was interpreted as the result of erroneous cell division, while the others seemed to be ‘‘mere’’ transitional stages, necessary for a return to the normal singlet. A series of facts suggested to us a new inquiry into the nature of the doublet of O. bifaria: (a) the spontaneous reappearance of doublets in the cultures kept in the laboratory; (b) from 1972 until today, such an event has occurred repeatedly in the laboratory and has been found in natural samples, as well; (c) the five stages in succession are always the same and (d) their behaviour is always stage-specific (stage I with its straight swimming reveals immediately the appearance of doublets in a normal strain even at a small magnification).

A study of ultrastructural morphology (Banchetti et al., 1997) showed that stage I is a new and characteristic one, with respect to the structure of the normal singlet. It does not seem to be a random heteromorphic state given that it presupposes delicate and sophisticated cortico-cytoplasmic adjustments between the entities that form the doublet itself. In Fig. 6 the same minute letters indicate the same characters shown here below: (a) the front extremity is pointed (which implies profound and biologically costly remakings of the cortical section and of the underlying complex cytoskeletal armature); (b) the ventral surfaces of the two hemicells are each folded to form a deep longitudinal furrow, thus confirming further serious cytoskeletal integrations between the two hemicells; (c) the transversal section of the doublet (a four-pointed star) is extremely reduced; (d) the cirri, the propulsive organelles of hypotrichs, do not even succeed in

SlfXJGLET DOUBLET

nuc

limensiondl lije tridimensional disperse

Fig. 6. The comparison between singlets and doublets of O. bifaria; the relative morphologies, transversal sections and the tracks typical of the two forms are schematized on the left and on the right, respectively. The small letters indicate the characteristics of the doublet described in the text with the same letters.

touching the substrate (that is the reason why only swimming is possible); (e) they are redistributed in their position of ventral insertion, finally representing two

symmetrical thrust apparati, (f) the nuclear apparati (apparently normal in form, ultrastructure and DNA content), coexist in the same cytoplasmic volume.

< Fig. 7. The phenomenon of ‘‘O. bifaria doublet’’ is simplified for illustrative purposes to some of its typical aspects, to show how it is possible, by means of the study of behaviour, to study the adaptive biology of eukaryotic cells. x = cell biology: cell membrane fusion, cortical and cytoskeletal integration, nuclear coordination; y = organism biology: differential feeding, differential reproduction, differential behaviour; z = environment biology: different substrates, different conditions, different microhabitats.

This morphological study provides a picture of a state of the species that, far from being a meaningless oddity, seems to represent the ideal form for a swimming organism at low Reynolds numbers (Purcell, 1977) ‘‘to penetrate the water better’’. The subsequent study of the behaviour of doublet-stage I showed (Fig. 6, lower part) that (a) it, the only one among the five stages of the doublet, can only swim (while O. bifaria is a species typically adapted to movement on the bidimensional substrate, Ricci, 1989a), (b) it swims along a rectilinear axis (and not helicoidal like all ciliates, Ricci, 1990), (c) with an only rarely interrupted swim (5.1s vs. 1.1s of the controls), (d) has singular interaction reactions that include a brief recoil (< 0.1s), before restarting swimming in a direction very near to the previous one (a° = 40°). The doublet of O. bifaria is a highly differentiated and biologically very costly (because of its form, section and cortical arrangement) ‘‘biological object’’, ideal for punching water (pointed with reduced transversal section) with a swim (given that creeping is impossible) that is straight (never observed in other species of ciliates) and virtually uninterrupted (both in time and in space). We could suggest that the doublet of O. bifaria, is not simply an error of nature, but a differentiative state (which explains the constancy of the 5 successive stages).

This new interpretation places it in the context of the other three of the same species: the conjugating couple (Ricci et al., 1975; Ricci and Banchetti, 1981), the cyst (Ricci et al., 1985), the carnivore giants (Ricci and Banchetti, 1993). The adaptive meaning that the doublet might have, in the light of this new series of experimental studies, might be that of mediating the dispersion of the population in the search for possible new spaces to colonize both actively (with a straight, constant swim in all possible directions in space), and passively, leaving the environment of almost still waters typical of the species, until the flow of the water current above is reached. This hypothesis finds a confirmation in the analogy that the ‘‘tomites’’ of Tetrahymena rostrata, interpreted by Corliss (1973) precisely as forms of dispersion, have with the doublets of O. bifaria. From the analysis of the different relevance of the doublestage I (Fig.7) we achieved an integrated reading of them: the double of O. bifaria very likely represents an allomorphic state of the species, periodically acquired by it for an adaptive purpose, the dispersion of the population in the environment (Ricci et al., 2000).

In conclusion, the study of the behaviour of protozoa has proved to have a heuristic value: (a) their uniqueness as eukaryotic ‘‘cell-organisms’’ made possible an explicit neuro-ethological approach, suggesting the actual existence of the hypothesized plan, on which a biologic-organismic reading (the

behaviour) can correspond directly to a biologic-cellular reading (the state of the potential of the membrane); (b) the study of the behaviour of protozoa, coupled to the peculiarities of their environment, so anti-intuitive for us, animals of large dimensions, made possible a considerably broader understanding of such an environment, together with a reading of its own qualities, faithfully revealed by the behavioural patterns themselves. The eco-ethological approach has shown that the behaviour represents a privileged biological trait capable of connecting organismic and environmental phenomena.

On the basis of this interdisciplinary approach to the specific aspects of the phenomenon ‘‘behaviour of protozoa’’, the concept of ‘‘adaptive space’’ for protozoa was proposed (and used in the case of the doublet of O. bifaria). The biological phenomena can be ‘‘broken down’’ into single cellular, organismic and environmental components, directly comparable and integrable with each other. In particular, what is of singular importance because of its biological uniqueness which allows us to pass from the biologic-cellular to the environmental components, thus proceeding to a direct reading in an adaptive key of phenomena typically linked to the cellular-eukaryotic arrangement.

The adaptive space hypothesis makes it possible to integrate cellular, organismic and environmental information in biologic adaptive conclusions.

References

Armentano T.V. 1980. Drainage of organic soils as a factor in the world carbon cycle. Bioscience. 30, 825830.

Azam F., Fenchel T., Field J.G., Gray J.S., Meyer-Reil L.A. and Thingstad F. 1983. The ecological role of water-column microbes in the sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 10, 257-263.

Banchetti R., Bisogni S. and Ricci N. 1997. The morphology of the doublet of Oxytricha bifaria: further studies. Cytobios. 90, 203-218.

Banchetti R. and Ricci N. 1986. The doublet of Oxytricha bifaria (Ciliata, Hypotrichida). I. Morphology and development. Protistologica.22, 2, 161-168.

Berg T.O. and Sand O. 1994. Spontaneous all-or-nothing action potentials in the ciliate Bursaridium difficile. J. Euk. Miccrobiol. 41, 13-17.

Cliff D. 1991. Computational neuroethology: a provisional manifesto. In: From animals to animats (Eds. Meyer J.A. and Wilson S.W.). MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. pp. 29-39.

Corliss J.O. 1973. History, taxonomy, ecology and evolution of species of Tetrahymena. In: Biology of

Tetrahymena (Ed. Elliott A.M.). Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross Inc, Stroudsburg, Pensylvania. pp. 1-55.

Ewert J.P. 1976. Neuro-Ethologie. Springer, Berlin.

Fenchel T. 1987. Ecology of protozoa. Science Tech. Publishers, Madison, Wisconsin and Springer-Verlag, Berlin.

Finlay B.J. 1990. Physiological ecology of free-living protozoa. Adv. Microb. Ecol. 11, 1-35.

Finlay B.J., Clarke K.J., Cowling A.J., Hindle R.M. and Rogerson A. 1988. On the abundance and distribution of protozoa and their food in a productive freshwater pond. Eur. J. Protistol. 23, 205-217.

Gould S.J., Raup D.M., Sepkoski J.J.Jr., Schopf T.J.M. and Simberloff D.S. 1977. The shape of evolution. A comparison of real and random clades. Paleobiology. 3, 173-181.

Gray R.H. 1995. Evaluation of fish behavior as a means of pollution monitoring. In: Pollution and Biomonitoring (Ed. Rana B.C.).Tata McGraw-Hill Publ. Co. Limtd., New Delhi. pp. 336-361.

Hoyle G. 1970. Cellular mechanisms underlying behavior-neuroethology. In: Adv. Ins. Physiol. Vol. 7 (Eds. Treherne J.E., Berridge M.J. and Wigglesworth W.B.). Academic Press, New York. pp. 349- 444.

Huber F. and Markl H. 1983. Preface. In: Neuroethology and behavioral physiology (Eds. Huber F. and Markl H.). Springer-Verlag, Berlin.

Jennings H.S. 1906. Behavior of the lower organisms. Bloomington, Indiana Univ. Press, London.

Kandel E.R. 1976. Cellular basis of behavior. Freeman, San Francisco.

Kittredge J.S. 1980. Behavioral bioassays: the range of response thresholds. Rapp. P.-v. Reun. Cons. int. Explor. Mer. 179, 212-227.

Leonildi A., Erra F., Banchetti R. and Ricci N. 1998. The ethograms of Uronychia transfuga and Uronychia setigera (Ciliata, Hypotrichida): a comparative approach for new insights into the behaviour of Protozoa. Eur. J. Protistol. 34, 426-435.

Lueken W., Ricci N. and Kruppel T. 1996. Rhythmic spontaneous depolarizations determine a slow-and-fast rhythm in walking of the marine hypotrich Euplotes vannus. Eur. J. Protistol. 32, 47-54.

Machemer H. and Teunis P.F.M. 1996. Sensory-motor coupling and motor responses. In: Ciliates: cells as organisms (Eds. Hausmann K. and Bradbury P.C.). Gustav Fischer Verlag, Stuttgart. pp. 379-402.

Mugnaini D., Ricci N., Banchetti R. and Kovacs P. 1995. Insulin treatment affects the behavior of Tetrahymena pyriformis and of T. malaccensis. Cytobios. 81, 87- 95.

Olla B.L., Pearson W.H. and Studholme A.L. 1980. Applicability ofbehavioral measures in environmental

stress assessment. Rapp. P.-v. Reun. Cons. int. Explor. Mer. 179, 1162-1173.

Purcell E.M. 1977. Life at low Reynolds number. Amer. J. Phys. 45, 3-11.

Ricci N. 1981.The ethogram of Oxytricha bifaria (Ciliata, Hypotrichida). I. The motile behavior. Acta Protozool. 20, 4, 393-410.

Ricci N. 1989. Locomotion as a criterion to read the adaptive biology of Protozoa and their evolution toward Metazoa. Boll Zool. 56, 245-263.

iНе можете найти то, что вам нужно? Попробуйте сервис подбора литературы.

Ricci N. 1989a. Microhabitats of Ciliates: specific adaptations to different substrates. Limnol. Oceanogr. 34, 6, 1089-1097.

Ricci N. 1990. The behaviour of ciliated Protozoa. Anim. Behav. 40, 1048-1069.

Ricci N. 1992. Neuroethology of ciliated protozoa: liceity and convenience of a terminological extension, relevance of a conceptual extrapolation. 3rd Int.Congr. of Neuroethology. McGill University, Montreal, Canada, p.79.

Ricci N. 1992a. Etho-ecology of Ciliates: a reappraisal of their adaptive biology, an insight in their environmental constraints. Acta Protozool. 31, 1, 19-32.

Ricci N. 1995. Protozoa: Bioindicators of environmental monitoring and pollution control. In: Pollution and Biomonitoring (Ed. Rana B.C.).Tata McGraw-Hill Publ. Co. Limtd., New Delhi. pp. 240-269.

Ricci N. 1996. Ethology of Ciliates. In: Ciliates: cells as organisms (Eds. Hausmann K. and Bradbury P.C.), Gustav Fischer Verlag, Stuttgart. pp. 403-416.

Ricci N., Banchetti R., Esposito F. and Nobili R. 1975. Conjugation in Oxytricha sp. (Hypotrichida, Ciliata). I. Morphocytological aspects. Acta Protozool. 13, 335-342.

Ricci N. and Banchetti R. 1981. Nuclear phenomena of vegetative and sexual reproduction in Oxytricha bifaria Stokes (Ciliata, Hypotrichida). Acta Protozool. 20, 2, 153-164.

Ricci N. and Banchetti R. 1993. The peculiar case of the giants of Oxytricha bifaria (Ciliata, Hypotrichida): a paradigmatic example of cell differentiation and adaptive strategy. Zool. Sc. 10, 393-410.

Ricci N. and Erra F. 1995.The crowding effect: an ethologic analysis. Eur. J. Protistol. 31, 302-308.

Ricci N., Erra F., Russo A. and Banchetti R. 1991. The air-water interface. A microhabitat for hypo-trichous settlers (Protista, Ciliata). Limnol. Oceanogr. 36, 6, 1178-1188.

Ricci N., Erra F., Russo A. and Banchetti R. 1992. The interface between air and water: a perturbation source eliciting adaptive behavior in Ciliates. J. Protozool. 39, 4, 521-525.

Ricci N., Erra F. and Banchetti R. 2000. The behavior of the doublet of Oxytricha bifaria (Ciliata,

Hypotrichida): a contribution to the understanding of this enigmatic form. J. Euk. Microbiol. 47, 2, 156-160.

Ricci N., Luvera G., Cacciatori M., Banchetti R. and Lueken W. 1997. The effects of 2^M Hg++ on the ethogram of Euplotes vannus (Ciliata, Hypotrichida). Eur. J. Protistol. 33, 63-71.

Ricci N., Rosati G. and Verni F. 1985. The cyst of Oxytricha bifaria (Ciliata, Hypotrichida). I. Mor-

phology and significance. Trans. Amer. Microsc. Soc. 104, 70-78.

Ricci N., Russo A., Banchetti R. and Kovacs P. 1995. The ethograms of Tetrahymena pyriformis GL and T. malaccensis, Cytobios. 83, 139-158.

Usherwood P.N.R. and Newth D.R. 1975. “Simple” nervous systems. Arnold, London.

Address for correspondence: Rosalba Banchetti, via A. Volta, 6 - 56126 Pisa - Italy. E-mail: [email protected]

The manuscript is presented by S.I. Fokin and A.A. Dobrovolskij

i Надоели баннеры? Вы всегда можете отключить рекламу.